Suppr超能文献

运动诱导性失明与微眼跳:因果关系

Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: cause and effect.

作者信息

Bonneh Yoram S, Donner Tobias H, Sagi Dov, Fried Moshe, Cooperman Alexander, Heeger David J, Arieli Amos

机构信息

Department of Human Biology, University of Haifa, Mt Carmel, Israel.

出版信息

J Vis. 2010 Dec 20;10(14):22. doi: 10.1167/10.14.22.

Abstract

It has been suggested that subjective disappearance of visual stimuli results from a spontaneous reduction of microsaccade rate causing image stabilization, enhanced adaptation, and a consequent fading. In motion-induced blindness (MIB), salient visual targets disappear intermittently when surrounded by a moving pattern. We investigated whether changes in microsaccade rate can account for MIB. We first determined that the moving mask does not affect microsaccade metrics (rate, magnitude, and temporal distribution). We then compared the dynamics of microsaccades during reported illusory disappearance (MIB) and physical disappearance (Replay) of a salient peripheral target. We found large modulations of microsaccade rate following perceptual transitions, whether illusory (MIB) or real (Replay). For MIB, the rate also decreased prior to disappearance and increased prior to reappearance. Importantly, MIB persisted in the presence of microsaccades although sustained microsaccade rate was lower during invisible than visible periods. These results suggest that the microsaccade system reacts to changes in visibility, but microsaccades also modulate MIB. The latter modulation is well described by a Poisson model of the perceptual transitions assuming that the probability for reappearance and disappearance is modulated following a microsaccade. Our results show that microsaccades counteract disappearance but are neither necessary nor sufficient to account for MIB.

摘要

有人提出,视觉刺激的主观消失是由于微扫视速率的自发降低导致图像稳定、适应性增强以及随之而来的消退。在运动诱导失明(MIB)中,当显著的视觉目标被移动模式包围时,会间歇性地消失。我们研究了微扫视速率的变化是否可以解释MIB。我们首先确定移动掩模不会影响微扫视指标(速率、幅度和时间分布)。然后,我们比较了在显著外周目标的报告性虚幻消失(MIB)和物理消失(重放)期间微扫视的动态变化。我们发现,无论是虚幻的(MIB)还是真实的(重放)感知转换后,微扫视速率都有很大的调制。对于MIB,速率在消失前也会降低,在重新出现前会增加。重要的是,尽管在不可见期间持续的微扫视速率低于可见期间,但MIB在存在微扫视的情况下仍然持续。这些结果表明,微扫视系统对可见性的变化做出反应,但微扫视也会调节MIB。假设重新出现和消失的概率在微扫视后受到调制,那么感知转换的泊松模型很好地描述了后一种调制。我们的结果表明,微扫视会抵消消失,但对于解释MIB既不是必要的也不是充分的。

相似文献

1
Motion-induced blindness and microsaccades: cause and effect.
J Vis. 2010 Dec 20;10(14):22. doi: 10.1167/10.14.22.
2
Motion-aftereffect-induced blindness.
J Vis. 2009 Oct 13;9(11):11.1-7. doi: 10.1167/9.11.11.
3
Microsaccade directions do not predict directionality of illusory brightness changes of overlapping transparent surfaces.
Vision Res. 2006 Oct;46(22):3823-30. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.007. Epub 2006 Aug 24.
4
Microsaccade rate varies with subjective visibility during motion-induced blindness.
PLoS One. 2009;4(4):e5163. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005163. Epub 2009 Apr 9.
5
Neural correlates of motion-induced blindness in the human brain.
J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Jun;22(6):1235-43. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21262.
7
A common mechanism for perceptual filling-in and motion-induced blindness.
Vision Res. 2006 Jun;46(12):1973-81. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.11.004. Epub 2005 Dec 27.
8
Motion-induced blindness and Troxler fading: common and different mechanisms.
PLoS One. 2014 Mar 21;9(3):e92894. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092894. eCollection 2014.
9
Oscillatory modulations in human fusiform cortex during motion-induced blindness: intracranial recording.
Clin Neurophysiol. 2012 Oct;123(10):1925-30. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.085. Epub 2012 Apr 12.
10
Linking motion-induced blindness to perceptual filling-in.
Vision Res. 2004 Nov;44(24):2857-66. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.029.

引用本文的文献

1
A compendium of considerations for methods in motion-induced blindness research.
Behav Res Methods. 2025 Aug 13;57(9):259. doi: 10.3758/s13428-025-02789-x.
2
Factors contributing to transient-induced fading: Examining the impact of luminance contrasts and subjective contours.
Iperception. 2024 Oct 30;15(5):20416695241290462. doi: 10.1177/20416695241290462. eCollection 2024 Sep-Oct.
3
Oculomotor inhibition markers of working memory load.
Sci Rep. 2024 Jan 22;14(1):1872. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-52518-1.
5
Fixation-related saccadic inhibition in free viewing in response to stimulus saliency.
Sci Rep. 2022 Apr 22;12(1):6619. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-10605-1.
6
Oculo-retinal dynamics can explain the perception of minimal recognizable configurations.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2021 Aug 24;118(34). doi: 10.1073/pnas.2022792118.
7
VisME: Visual Microsaccades Explorer.
J Eye Mov Res. 2019 Dec 12;12(6). doi: 10.16910/jemr.12.6.5.
9
Closed loop motor-sensory dynamics in human vision.
PLoS One. 2020 Oct 15;15(10):e0240660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240660. eCollection 2020.

本文引用的文献

1
Rare but precious: microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation.
Vision Res. 2010 Jun 11;50(12):1173-84. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.04.007. Epub 2010 Apr 9.
2
3
Microsaccades: small steps on a long way.
Vision Res. 2009 Oct;49(20):2415-41. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.010. Epub 2009 Aug 13.
4
Microsaccade rate varies with subjective visibility during motion-induced blindness.
PLoS One. 2009;4(4):e5163. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005163. Epub 2009 Apr 9.
6
Motion-induced blindness and motion streak suppression.
Curr Biol. 2009 Feb 24;19(4):325-9. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.12.053. Epub 2009 Feb 12.
7
The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control.
J Vis. 2008 Dec 18;8(14):20.1-21. doi: 10.1167/8.14.20.
8
Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in.
J Vis. 2008 Nov 4;8(14):15.1-9. doi: 10.1167/8.14.15.
9
Visibility states modulate microsaccade rate and direction.
Vision Res. 2009 Jan;49(2):228-36. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.10.015. Epub 2008 Nov 26.
10
Opposite neural signatures of motion-induced blindness in human dorsal and ventral visual cortex.
J Neurosci. 2008 Oct 8;28(41):10298-310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2371-08.2008.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验