Suppr超能文献

急性呼吸道感染的认知反思与抗生素处方

Cognitive reflection and antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory infections.

作者信息

Pineros Dwan B, Doctor Jason N, Friedberg Mark W, Meeker Daniella, Linder Jeffrey A

机构信息

Geisel School of Medicine at Dartmouth, Hanover, NH, USA.

Leonard D. Schaeffer Center for Health Policy and Economics, School of Pharmacy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA.

出版信息

Fam Pract. 2016 Jun;33(3):309-11. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmw015. Epub 2016 Mar 21.

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Variation in clinical decision-making could be explained by clinicians' tendency to make 'snap-decisions' versus making more reflective decisions. One common clinical decision with unexplained variation is the prescription of antibiotics for acute respiratory infections (ARIs).

OBJECTIVE

We hypothesized that clinicians who tended toward greater cognitive reflection would be less likely to prescribe antibiotics for ARIs.

METHODS

The Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) is a psychological test with three questions with intuitive but incorrect answers that respondents reach if they do not consider the question carefully. The CRT is scored from 0 to 3, representing the number of correct answers. A higher score indicates greater cognitive reflection. We administered the CRT to 187 clinicians in 50 primary care practices. From billing and electronic health record data, we calculated clinician-level antibiotic prescribing rates for ARIs in 3 categories: all ARIs, antibiotic-appropriate ARIs and non-antibiotic-appropriate ARIs.

RESULTS

A total of 57 clinicians (31%) scored 0 points on the CRT; 38 (20%) scored 1; 51 (27%) scored 2; and 41 (22%) scored 3. We found a roughly U-shaped association between cognitive reflection and antibiotic prescribing. The antibiotic prescribing rate for CRT scores of 0, 1, 2 and 3 for all ARIs (n = 37080 visits) was 32%, 26%, 25% and 30% (P = 0.10); for antibiotic-appropriate ARIs (n = 11220 visits) was 60%, 55%, 54% and 58% (P = 0.63); and for non-antibiotic-appropriate ARIs (n = 25860 visits) was 21%, 17%, 13% and 18%, respectively (P = 0.03).

CONCLUSIONS

In contrast to our hypothesis, there appears to be a 'sweet-spot' of cognitive reflection for antibiotic prescribing for non-antibiotic-appropriate ARIs. Differences in clinicians' cognitive reflection may be associated with other variations in care.

摘要

背景

临床决策的差异可能是由于临床医生倾向于做出“快速决策”而非进行更深入的思考。急性呼吸道感染(ARI)抗生素处方是存在无法解释差异的常见临床决策之一。

目的

我们假设认知反思能力较强的临床医生为ARI开具抗生素的可能性较小。

方法

认知反思测试(CRT)是一项心理测试,包含三个问题,答案直观但不正确,如果受访者不仔细思考问题就会得出这些答案。CRT的评分从0到3,代表正确答案的数量。分数越高表明认知反思能力越强。我们对50家基层医疗诊所的187名临床医生进行了CRT测试。根据计费和电子健康记录数据,我们计算了临床医生层面三类ARI的抗生素处方率:所有ARI、适合使用抗生素的ARI和不适合使用抗生素的ARI。

结果

共有57名临床医生(31%)在CRT测试中得0分;38名(20%)得1分;51名(27%)得2分;41名(22%)得3分。我们发现认知反思与抗生素处方之间存在大致呈U形的关联。对于所有ARI(n = 37080次就诊),CRT评分为0、1、2和3时的抗生素处方率分别为32%、26%、25%和30%(P = 0.10);对于适合使用抗生素的ARI(n = 11220次就诊),分别为60%、55%、54%和58%(P = 0.63);对于不适合使用抗生素的ARI(n = 25860次就诊),分别为21%、17%、13%和18%(P = 0.03)。

结论

与我们的假设相反,对于不适合使用抗生素的ARI,抗生素处方似乎存在一个认知反思的“最佳点”。临床医生认知反思的差异可能与其他护理差异有关。

相似文献

1
Cognitive reflection and antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory infections.
Fam Pract. 2016 Jun;33(3):309-11. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmw015. Epub 2016 Mar 21.
2
Behavioral interventions to reduce inappropriate antibiotic prescribing: a randomized pilot trial.
BMC Infect Dis. 2016 Aug 5;16:373. doi: 10.1186/s12879-016-1715-8.
3
Necessity of office visits for acute respiratory infections in primary care.
Fam Pract. 2016 Jun;33(3):312-7. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmw019. Epub 2016 Apr 5.
5
Antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory infections in children in Jordan.
Int Health. 2017 Mar 1;9(2):124-130. doi: 10.1093/inthealth/ihx003.
7
Empathy, burnout, and antibiotic prescribing for acute respiratory infections: a cross-sectional primary care study in the US.
Br J Gen Pract. 2017 Aug;67(661):e565-e571. doi: 10.3399/bjgp17X691901. Epub 2017 Jul 17.

引用本文的文献

2
Behavioral Economics and Ambulatory Antibiotic Stewardship: A Narrative Review.
Clin Ther. 2021 Oct;43(10):1654-1667. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2021.08.004. Epub 2021 Oct 23.
3
Cognitive bias: how understanding its impact on antibiotic prescribing decisions can help advance antimicrobial stewardship.
JAC Antimicrob Resist. 2020 Dec 21;2(4):dlaa107. doi: 10.1093/jacamr/dlaa107. eCollection 2020 Dec.
5
Moving the mean with feedback: insights from behavioural science.
NPJ Prim Care Respir Med. 2016 Apr 28;26:16018. doi: 10.1038/npjpcrm.2016.18.

本文引用的文献

1
The time course of conflict on the Cognitive Reflection Test.
Cognition. 2016 May;150:109-18. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.015. Epub 2016 Feb 17.
3
Exogenous cortisol causes a shift from deliberative to intuitive thinking.
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016 Feb;64:131-5. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.11.018. Epub 2015 Nov 24.
4
Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition?
Behav Res Methods. 2016 Mar;48(1):341-8. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0576-1.
6
Can exposure to prenatal sex hormones (2D:4D) predict cognitive reflection?
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014 May;43:1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.023. Epub 2014 Feb 6.
7
The etiology of diagnostic errors: a controlled trial of system 1 versus system 2 reasoning.
Acad Med. 2014 Feb;89(2):277-84. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000105.
8
Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach.
Mem Cognit. 2014 Apr;42(3):434-47. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0367-9.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验