Johnson Sarah A, Painter Michele S, Javurek Angela B, Murphy Claire R, Howald Emily C, Khan Zoya Z, Conard Caroline M, Gant Kristal L, Ellersieck Mark R, Hoffmann Frauke, Schenk A Katrin, Rosenfeld Cheryl S
Bond Life Sciences Center and Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Missouri.
Agriculture Experimental Station-Statistics, University of Missouri.
J Comp Psychol. 2017 Feb;131(1):30-39. doi: 10.1037/com0000057.
Rodent species, such as monogamous and biparental California mice, produce vocalizations as a means of communication. A temporal examination of vocalizations produced by California mice pups in isolation was performed. Pup recordings were performed for 3 min at ∼10.00 and 14.00 hrs on early postnatal days (PND) 2-4, 7, 21, and 28. Once initial recordings were finished, pups were returned to the home cage with parents and any siblings for 5 minutes to determine if active biparental responses resulted in an enhanced vocalization response when pups were isolated and retested. We also sought to determine whether potential reduction in vocalizations by older pups might be due to procedure-habituation procedure associated with less anxiety and/or possibly decreased need for parental care. Vocalizations were measured in weanling (30 days of age) "naïve" pups not previously isolated. Results show older pups took significantly longer to vocalize, indicated by increased latency before producing their initial syllable compared to earlier ages. With increasing age, pups demonstrated decreased syllable duration, reduced number and duration of phrases, and decreased number of syllables per phrase. No differences in pup vocalizations were observed before and after being placed back with parents, suggestive biparental potentiation may not exist in California mice pups. Comparison of the naïve to habituated weanling pups indicated the former group had more total calls but no other differences in vocalization parameters were detected between these 2 groups. Collectively, the findings suggest that as California mice pups mature and approach weaning they generally vocalize less in isolation. (PsycINFO Database Record
啮齿动物物种,如实行一夫一妻制且双亲共同抚养后代的加利福尼亚小鼠,会发出叫声作为一种交流方式。对单独饲养的加利福尼亚小鼠幼崽发出的叫声进行了时间分析。在出生后早期(出生后第2 - 4天、第7天、第21天和第28天)的上午10:00和下午2:00对幼崽进行3分钟的录音。一旦初始录音完成,幼崽会被放回与父母及任何兄弟姐妹共处的笼子里5分钟,以确定当幼崽被隔离并重新测试时,双亲积极的反应是否会导致叫声反应增强。我们还试图确定大龄幼崽叫声的潜在减少是否可能是由于与焦虑减少和/或对父母照顾需求可能降低相关的程序习惯化过程。在之前未被隔离的断奶(30日龄)“未经历过的”幼崽中测量叫声。结果显示,与较小年龄相比,大龄幼崽发出叫声的时间明显更长,这表现为发出初始音节前的延迟增加。随着年龄增长,幼崽的音节持续时间缩短,短语数量和持续时间减少,每个短语中的音节数量也减少。将幼崽放回父母身边前后,未观察到叫声有差异,这表明加利福尼亚小鼠幼崽可能不存在双亲增强效应。将未经历过的断奶幼崽与习惯化的断奶幼崽进行比较,结果表明前一组的总叫声更多,但在这两组之间未检测到叫声参数的其他差异。总体而言,研究结果表明,随着加利福尼亚小鼠幼崽成熟并接近断奶期,它们在单独状态下通常叫声会减少。(《心理学文摘数据库记录》