School of Kinesiology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada.
Graduate Program in Neuroscience, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada.
J Neurotrauma. 2020 Dec 1;37(23):2558-2568. doi: 10.1089/neu.2019.6865. Epub 2020 Jul 8.
A sport-related concussion (SRC) results in short- and long-term deficits in oculomotor control; however, it is unclear whether this change reflects executive dysfunction and/or a performance decrement caused by an increase in task-based symptom burden. Here, individuals with a SRC - and age- and sex-matched controls - completed an antisaccade task (i.e., saccade mirror-symmetrical to a target) during the early (initial assessment ≤12 days) and later (follow-up assessment <30 days) stages of recovery. Antisaccades were used because they require top-down executive control and exhibit performance decrements following an SRC. Reaction time (RT) and directional errors were included with pupillometry, because pupil size in the antisaccade task has been shown to provide a neural proxy for executive control. In addition, the Sport-Concussion Assessment Tool (SCAT-5) symptom checklist was completed prior to and after each oculomotor assessment to identify a possible task-based increase in symptomology. The SRC group yielded longer initial assessment RTs, more directional errors, and larger task-evoked pupil dilations (TEPD) than the control group. At the follow-up assessment, RTs for the SRC and control group did not reliably differ; however, the former demonstrated more directional errors and larger TEPDs. SCAT-5 symptom severity scores did not vary from the pre- to post-oculomotor evaluation for either initial or follow-up assessments. Accordingly, an SRC imparts a persistent executive dysfunction to oculomotor planning independent of a task-based increase in symptom burden. These findings evince that antisaccades serve as an effective tool to identify subtle executive deficits during the early and later stages of SRC recovery.
运动相关性脑震荡 (SRC) 会导致眼球运动控制的短期和长期缺陷;然而,目前尚不清楚这种变化是否反映了执行功能障碍,或者是否是由于任务相关症状负担增加而导致的表现下降。在这里,患有 SRC 的个体 - 以及年龄和性别匹配的对照组 - 在恢复的早期(初始评估≤12 天)和晚期(随访评估<30 天)阶段完成了反扫视任务(即,与目标镜面对称的扫视)。选择反扫视是因为它们需要自上而下的执行控制,并且在 SRC 后表现出下降。反应时间 (RT) 和方向错误与瞳孔测量一起包括在内,因为反扫视任务中的瞳孔大小已被证明提供了执行控制的神经代理。此外,在每次眼动评估之前和之后,都完成了运动性脑震荡评估工具 (SCAT-5) 症状检查表,以确定可能存在基于任务的症状增加。SRC 组的初始评估 RT 较长、方向错误较多且任务诱发的瞳孔扩张 (TEPD) 较大,对照组的 RT 较短、方向错误较少且 TEPD 较小。在随访评估中,SRC 和对照组的 RT 没有可靠地差异;然而,前者表现出更多的方向错误和更大的 TEPD。在初始或随访评估中,SCAT-5 症状严重程度评分都没有从眼动评估前到后发生变化。因此,SRC 会对眼球运动计划产生持久的执行功能障碍,而与任务相关的症状负担增加无关。这些发现表明,反扫视是一种有效的工具,可以在 SRC 恢复的早期和晚期阶段识别出微妙的执行缺陷。