National Marine Mammal Foundation, San Diego, CA, United States of America.
PLoS One. 2020 May 22;15(5):e0233658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233658. eCollection 2020.
Most commonly, animal communication systems are driven by shared call repertoires, with some individual distinctiveness encoded as a byproduct of voice cues. We provide evidence that bottlenose dolphins produce both individually distinctive whistles, and a shared whistle type. A stereotyped whistle contour (termed the group whistle) is shared by five bottlenose dolphins that have lived, worked, and traveled together for at least 21 years. These five dolphins are members of a group of eight dolphins that work as a specialized team for the Navy Marine Mammal Program. Each dolphin is routinely recorded during periods when an individual is isolated from the others in above ground pools as part of their routine training. Each of the eight dolphins has an individually distinctive signature whistle. In addition, at least five of these dolphins share a distinct non-signature whistle type. This shared whistle contour was produced an average of 22.4% +/- 9.0% of the time during periods in which individuals were isolated. During these isolations the signature whistle was produced an average of 42.9% +/- 11.9% of the time. This is consistent with decades of signature whistle research. A group of 10 naïve observers rated the similarity of the different whistle contours. The observers rated the group whistle contour produced by all five dolphins as highly similar (P < 0.01). Their ratings further showed that the signature whistles of the five dolphins were very different (P < 0.01). These findings were further supported by discriminant function analyses. That said, the shared whistle contours still exhibited individual differences which may allow conspecifics to identify the producer even when a whistle contour is shared among multiple dolphins. This is the first in-depth analysis of a non-signature whistle type shared among multiple conspecifics.
通常情况下,动物的交流系统是由共享的叫声组成的,其中一些个体的独特性是通过声音线索编码的副产品。我们提供的证据表明,宽吻海豚既能发出个体独特的哨声,也能发出共享的哨声类型。一种刻板的哨声轮廓(称为群体哨声)被五头宽吻海豚共享,这五头海豚一起生活、工作和旅行了至少 21 年。这五头海豚是海军哺乳动物计划一个特殊团队的八头海豚中的成员。每头海豚在个体与其他海豚隔离的时期,都会在地上的水池中进行例行记录,作为其日常训练的一部分。这八头海豚中的每一头都有自己独特的标志性哨声。此外,其中至少有五头海豚还共享一种独特的非标志性哨声类型。在个体隔离期间,这种共享的哨声轮廓平均出现了 22.4% +/- 9.0%的时间。在这些隔离期间,标志性哨声平均出现了 42.9% +/- 11.9%的时间。这与几十年的标志性哨声研究一致。一组 10 名不知情的观察者对不同哨声轮廓的相似性进行了评分。观察者对所有五头海豚发出的群体哨声轮廓的评分非常相似(P < 0.01)。他们的评分进一步表明,这五头海豚的标志性哨声非常不同(P < 0.01)。判别函数分析进一步支持了这些发现。也就是说,共享的哨声轮廓仍然表现出个体差异,即使在多个海豚共享哨声轮廓的情况下,同类个体也可以识别出发出哨声的个体。这是首次对多个同类个体共享的非标志性哨声类型进行深入分析。