Suppr超能文献

人们对不成比例的疼痛的生理和心理解释持混合反应。

People Have Mixed Reactions to Both Physiological and Psychological Explanations of Disproportionate Pain.

机构信息

Department of Surgery and Perioperative Care, Dell Medical School, the University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

出版信息

Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2022 Jul 1;480(7):1387-1398. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002163. Epub 2022 Mar 8.

Abstract

BACKGROUND

There are two general frameworks that conceptualize pain that is more intense or persistent than expected based on measurable pathologic findings: the psychological (unhelpful thoughts and emotions) and the physiological (purported nervous system dysfunction, such as central sensitization). Some clinicians believe people will be more receptive to a physiological conceptualization. Prior quantitative research demonstrated that carefully crafted psychological explanations are rated similarly to crafted physiological explanations, with relatively mixed reactions. This qualitative study was undertaken in parallel with that quantitative study to help develop effective communication and treatment strategies by identifying specific thoughts and feelings (themes) regarding the physiological and psychological conceptualizations of disproportionate pain that make people more or less comfortable considering comprehensive, biopsychosocial treatment approaches.

QUESTION/PURPOSE: What themes arise in patient thoughts and feelings regarding physiological and psychological conceptualizations of pain that is more intense or persistent than expected?

METHODS

We sought to understand the experience of considering pain as a biopsychosocial experience (phenomenology approach) by studying the thoughts and feelings that arise as people seeking care for arm and back pain engage with physiological and psychological conceptualizations of pain that is more intense or persistent than one would expect based on the pathology. We recruited 29 patients presenting for upper extremity or back pain specialty care at one of two urban offices, intentionally recruiting people of various ages, genders, backgrounds, socioeconomic status, as well as type and duration of pain (purposive sampling). The 29 patients included 18 women and 11 men (16 married, 15 non-White, 20 with arm pain) with a median (interquartile range) age of 62 years (42 to 67). The interviews were conducted by a trained woman orthopaedic surgeon interviewer using a semistructured interview guide soliciting participants' thoughts and feelings about a physiological explanation (nerves in the central nervous system stuck in the on position can make pain more intense) and a psychological explanation (unhelpful thoughts and feelings of distress can make pain more intense) for pain more intense or persistent than expected. The interviews were transcribed and themes were identified as the data were collected. Based on current experimental evidence, including what is known about the physiological effects of thoughts, feelings, and context (placebo/nocebo effects), we assumed an underlying physiological basis for pain that is variably experienced and expressed (mixed postpositive/interpretive approach). Themes were identified in the interview transcripts systematically by two coders and then discussed with the entire research team to arrive at consensus. We stopped enrolling patients when the authors agreed that additional themes did not arise in five consecutive interviews.

RESULTS

The following themes and interpretations were derived from the analysis: Neither the physiological nor the psychological explanation for disproportionate pain (1) avoided the stigma associated with mental health, (2) was consistently understood, (3) provided a consistent sense of control, (4) consistently provided hope, and (5) represented the stress and emotion of disproportionate or persistent pain. The physiological explanation also generated mixed reactions regarding whether or not it: (1) was a useful point of conversation, (2) was reassuring or frightening, and (3) supported physiological or psychological treatments. The psychological explanation made some people feel worse.

CONCLUSION

People have mixed reactions to both physiological and psychological explanations of disproportionate pain. As such, without direction on content, communication might be most effective by focusing on relational aspects, such as emotional connection and trust.

CLINICAL RELEVANCE

Although there is room to improve the content of strategies for explaining more pain than expected to patients, our findings extend the discoveries of others in highlighting the need for tailored relational communication strategies that prioritize feeling heard, validated, and accompanied.

摘要

背景

有两种普遍的框架可以用来理解基于可测量病理发现的比预期更强烈或更持久的疼痛:心理(无益的想法和情绪)和生理(据称的神经系统功能障碍,如中枢敏化)。一些临床医生认为,人们会更容易接受生理概念化。先前的定量研究表明,精心设计的心理解释与精心设计的生理解释评分相似,反应相对混合。这项定性研究与定量研究同时进行,旨在通过确定与不成比例的疼痛的生理和心理概念化相关的具体想法和感受(主题),帮助制定有效的沟通和治疗策略,这些主题可以让人更愿意或不愿意考虑全面的、生物心理社会的治疗方法。

问题/目的:关于比预期更强烈或更持久的疼痛的生理和心理概念化,患者的想法和感受中出现了哪些主题?

方法

我们通过研究寻求理解将疼痛视为生物心理体验的体验(现象学方法),研究了人们在寻求上肢和背部疼痛治疗时,对疼痛的生理和心理概念化的想法和感受,这些概念化比基于病理的预期更强烈或更持久。我们在两个城市办公室中的一个招募了 29 名上肢或背部疼痛专科就诊的患者,有意招募了各种年龄、性别、背景、社会经济地位以及疼痛类型和持续时间的患者(有目的的抽样)。29 名患者包括 18 名女性和 11 名男性(16 名已婚,15 名非白人,20 名上肢疼痛),中位(四分位间距)年龄为 62 岁(42 至 67 岁)。访谈由一名经过培训的女性骨科医生采访者使用半结构化访谈指南进行,征求参与者对生理解释(中枢神经系统中的神经卡在开启位置可能会使疼痛更剧烈)和心理解释(无益的思想和痛苦的感觉可能会使疼痛更剧烈)的想法和感受,这些解释用于比预期更强烈或更持久的疼痛。访谈被转录,并在收集数据时确定主题。基于当前的实验证据,包括关于思想、感觉和环境(安慰剂/反安慰剂效应)的生理效应的知识,我们假设疼痛具有可变性和表达(混合后正/解释性方法)的潜在生理基础。两位编码员系统地在访谈记录中识别主题,然后与整个研究团队讨论以达成共识。当作者同意在连续五次访谈中没有出现其他主题时,我们停止招募患者。

结果

从分析中得出了以下主题和解释:(1)不成比例的疼痛的生理或心理解释都没有避免与心理健康相关的污名化,(2)都能被一致理解,(3)都提供了一致的控制感,(4)都能提供希望,(5)都代表了不成比例或持续疼痛的压力和情绪。生理解释在是否:(1)是有用的讨论点,(2)是令人安心还是令人恐惧,以及(3)支持生理或心理治疗方面也产生了混合反应。心理解释让一些人感觉更糟。

结论

人们对不成比例疼痛的生理和心理解释有不同的反应。因此,在没有关于内容的指导的情况下,沟通可能通过关注情感联系和信任等关系方面最有效。

临床相关性

尽管有改进向患者解释比预期更强烈的疼痛的策略的内容的空间,但我们的发现扩展了其他人的发现,强调需要量身定制的关系沟通策略,这些策略优先考虑被倾听、被认可和被陪伴的感觉。

相似文献

1
People Have Mixed Reactions to Both Physiological and Psychological Explanations of Disproportionate Pain.
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2022 Jul 1;480(7):1387-1398. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002163. Epub 2022 Mar 8.
2
Can Crafted Communication Strategies Allow Musculoskeletal Specialists to Address Health Within the Biopsychosocial Paradigm?
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2021 Jun 1;479(6):1217-1223. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000001635.
3
How Do Orthopaedic Providers Conceptualize Good Patient Outcomes and Their Barriers and Facilitators After Acute Injury? A Qualitative Study.
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2023 Jun 1;481(6):1088-1100. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002473. Epub 2022 Nov 8.
5
Unhelpful Thoughts and Distress Regarding Symptoms Limit Accommodation of Musculoskeletal Pain.
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2022 Feb 1;480(2):276-283. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002006.
8
Does Addressing Mental Health During a Musculoskeletal Specialty Care Visit Affect Patient-rated Clinician Empathy?
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2023 May 1;481(5):976-983. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002494. Epub 2022 Dec 1.
9
Do Unhelpful Thoughts or Confidence in Problem Solving Have Stronger Associations with Musculoskeletal Illness?
Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2022 Feb 1;480(2):287-295. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000002005.

引用本文的文献

1
The Central Sensitization Inventory Measures Thoughts and Emotions.
J Patient Exp. 2024 Aug 14;11:23743735241273589. doi: 10.1177/23743735241273589. eCollection 2024.
2
Contralateral Arm Pain as a Sign of Distress Regarding Symptoms.
Hand (N Y). 2025 Mar;20(2):312-318. doi: 10.1177/15589447231216145. Epub 2023 Dec 11.
3
Do Items Addressing Thoughts and Emotions Regarding Symptoms Measure Distinct Aspects of Musculoskeletal Health?
J Patient Exp. 2023 Nov 6;10:23743735231211776. doi: 10.1177/23743735231211776. eCollection 2023.
4
Innovative treatment formats, technologies, and clinician trainings that improve access to behavioral pain treatment for youth and adults.
Front Pain Res (Lausanne). 2023 Jul 20;4:1223172. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2023.1223172. eCollection 2023.
5
Mental Health Among People Presenting for Care of Physical Symptoms: The Factors Associated with Suicidality and Symptoms of Depression and Anxiety are Similar Across Specialties.
Chronic Stress (Thousand Oaks). 2023 Apr 18;7:24705470231169106. doi: 10.1177/24705470231169106. eCollection 2023 Jan-Dec.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验