Department of Ecoepidemiology, Institute of Tropical Medicine (NEKKEN), Nagasaki University, Nagasaki, Japan.
Graduate School of Arts and Letters, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.
PLoS One. 2022 Nov 7;17(11):e0275855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0275855. eCollection 2022.
Voice messages have been employed as an effective and efficient approach for increasing health service utilization and health promotion in low- and middle-income countries. However, unlike SMS, voice message services require their users to pick up a phone call at its delivery time. Furthermore, voice messages are difficult for the users to review their contents afterward. While recognizing that voice messages are more friendly to specific groups (eg, illiterate or less literate populations), there should be several challenges in successfully operationalizing its intervention program.
This study is aimed to estimate the extent to which voice message service users pick up the phone calls of voice messages and complete listening up to or beyond the core part of voice messages.
A voice message service program composed of 14 episodes on maternal, newborn, and child health was piloted in Lagos, Nigeria, from 2018 to 2019. A voice message call of each of 14 episodes was delivered to the mobile phones of the program participants per day for 14 consecutive days. A total of 513 participants in the voice message service chose one of five locally spoken languages as the language to be used for voice messages. Two multilevel logistic regression models were created to understand participants' adherence to the voice message: (a) Model 1 for testing whether a voice message call is picked up; and (b) Model 2 for testing whether a voice message call having been picked up is listened to up to the core messaging part.
The greater the voice message episode number became, the smaller proportion of the participants picked up the phone calls of voice message (aOR: 0.98; 95% CI: 0.97-0.99; P = .01). Only 854 of 3765 voice message calls having been picked up by the participants (22.7%) were listened to up to their core message parts. It was found that picking up a phone call did not necessarily ensure listening up to the core message part. This indicates a discontinuity between these two actions.
The participants were likely to stop picking up the phone as the episode number of voice messages progressed. In view of the discontinuity between picking up a phone call and listening up to the core message part, we should not assume that those picking up the phone would automatically complete listening to the entire or core voice message.
在中低收入国家,语音信息已被用作增加卫生服务利用和促进健康的有效且高效的方法。然而,与短信不同,语音消息服务要求用户在发送时接听电话。此外,用户很难在之后查看语音消息的内容。虽然认识到语音消息对特定群体(例如,文盲或文化程度较低的人群)更友好,但在成功实施其干预计划方面仍存在一些挑战。
本研究旨在评估语音消息服务用户接听语音消息电话并完成收听核心内容或超出核心内容的程度。
2018 年至 2019 年,在尼日利亚拉各斯试点了一个由 14 集组成的母婴和儿童健康语音消息服务计划。该计划每天向参与者的手机发送 14 集的语音消息电话,持续 14 天。共有 513 名参与语音消息服务的参与者选择了五种当地语言中的一种作为语音消息的语言。创建了两个多层次逻辑回归模型来了解参与者对语音消息的遵守情况:(a)模型 1 用于测试语音消息电话是否被接听;(b)模型 2 用于测试已接听的语音消息电话是否被收听至核心消息部分。
语音消息集数越大,参与者接听语音消息电话的比例越小(aOR:0.98;95%CI:0.97-0.99;P =.01)。只有 3765 个由参与者接听的语音消息电话中的 854 个(22.7%)被收听至核心消息部分。结果表明,接听电话不一定能确保收听核心消息部分。这表明这两个动作之间存在不连续性。
随着语音消息集数的增加,参与者可能会停止接听电话。鉴于接听电话和收听核心消息部分之间的不连续性,我们不应假设接听电话的人会自动完整收听整个或核心语音消息。