Borta A, Wöhr M, Schwarting R K W
Experimental and Physiological Psychology Unit, Section for Experimental and Biological Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps-University of Marburg, Gutenbergstr. 18, 35032 Marburg, Germany.
Behav Brain Res. 2006 Jan 30;166(2):271-80. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.08.009. Epub 2005 Oct 4.
Our previous work has shown that male Wistar rats, although identical in breeder, age and housing conditions, can differ systematically in their anxiety-related behavior as measured in the elevated plus-maze. Since such individual dispositions can affect the responsiveness in other aversively motivated situations, we asked in a 1st experiment whether such rats might also differ in a test of conditioned fear. Based on their levels of spontaneous open arm avoidance in the elevated plus-maze, 20 adult male Wistar rats were divided into those with "high open arm" (HOA) versus "low open arm" (LOA) time. These rats were then tested in a standard fear conditioning paradigm. During the conditioning procedure, they received 6 tone (3 kHz, 20 s) and shock (0.5 mA, 0.5 s) pairings, each followed by a 60 s inter-stimulus interval. Conditioned responses to the tone were tested 24 h thereafter. During both days, freezing behavior and ultrasonic vocalization were measured. Differences in ultrasonic vocalization between HOA and LOA rats were detected during the conditioning day, where vocalization was more likely in LOA rats. Furthermore, LOA rats emitted calls with higher frequency components than HOA rats. On the subsequent day of testing, the number of animals vocalizing, and the rate of vocalization was decreased, and call differences between groups were no longer detectable. In freezing, differences between HOA and LOA rats were observed on the conditioning day, where LOA rats showed more freezing behavior during the tone/shock intervals. Also, on the test day, they showed more freezing behavior during the tone intervals compared to HOA rats. These results indicate that acute and conditioned responses of rats in a conventional fear conditioning paradigm can depend on individual dispositions of anxiety-related behavior as measured with the elevated plus-maze. In a 2nd experiment, we asked whether exposure to an elevated plus-maze would lead to ultrasonic vocalization, which we tested in rats which had been handled or non-handled prior to testing. Most importantly, we found that none of the animals displayed any vocalization in the plus-maze, neither during a 1st nor a repeated test 1 day later. These data are discussed with respect to the presumed role of ultrasonic vocalization in aversively motivated situations, and the mechanisms, which may account for the behavioral differences between HOA and LOA rats in such tests.
我们之前的研究表明,雄性Wistar大鼠尽管在繁殖者、年龄和饲养条件方面相同,但在高架十字迷宫中所测量的焦虑相关行为上可能存在系统性差异。由于这种个体倾向可能会影响在其他厌恶动机情境中的反应性,我们在第一个实验中询问,此类大鼠在条件性恐惧测试中是否也会存在差异。基于它们在高架十字迷宫中自发的开臂回避水平,20只成年雄性Wistar大鼠被分为“高开臂”(HOA)时间组和“低开臂”(LOA)时间组。然后,这些大鼠在标准恐惧条件化范式中接受测试。在条件化过程中,它们接受6次音调(3千赫兹,20秒)与电击(0.5毫安,0.5秒)配对,每次配对后有60秒的刺激间隔。此后24小时测试对音调的条件反应。在这两天中,测量了僵住行为和超声波发声。在条件化当天检测到HOA和LOA大鼠之间超声波发声的差异,LOA大鼠更有可能发声。此外,LOA大鼠发出的叫声频率成分高于HOA大鼠。在随后的测试日,发声的动物数量和发声率下降,且两组之间的叫声差异不再可检测到。在僵住方面,在条件化当天观察到HOA和LOA大鼠之间的差异,LOA大鼠在音调/电击间隔期间表现出更多的僵住行为。同样,在测试日,与HOA大鼠相比,它们在音调间隔期间表现出更多的僵住行为。这些结果表明,在传统恐惧条件化范式中,大鼠的急性和条件反应可能取决于用高架十字迷宫测量的焦虑相关行为的个体倾向。在第二个实验中,我们询问暴露于高架十字迷宫是否会导致超声波发声,我们在测试前经过处理或未经过处理的大鼠中对此进行了测试。最重要的是,我们发现没有一只动物在十字迷宫中发声,无论是在第一次测试还是一天后的重复测试中。我们结合超声波发声在厌恶动机情境中的假定作用以及可能解释HOA和LOA大鼠在此类测试中行为差异的机制来讨论这些数据。