Hermer-Vazquez Linda, Hermer-Vazquez Raymond, Chapin John K
SUNY Health Science Center, Brooklyn, NY 11203, United States.
Behav Brain Res. 2007 Feb 27;177(2):322-8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.029. Epub 2007 Jan 4.
Humans and non-human animals make use of sensory hierarchies in "selecting" strategies for solving many cognitive and behavioral tasks. Often, if a preferred type of sensory information is unavailable or is not useful for solving a given task, the animal can switch to a lower-priority strategy, making use of a different class of sensory information. In the case of rats performing a classic reach-to-grasp-food task, however, prior studies indicate that the reaching maneuver may be a fixed action pattern that is guided exclusively by the food's odor plume until the point of contact with the food morsel [Whishaw IQ, Tomie JA. Olfaction directs skilled forelimb reaching in the rat. Behav Brain Res 1989;32(1):11-21; Metz GA, Whishaw IQ. Skilled reaching an action pattern: stability in rat (Rattus norvegicus) grasping movements as a function of changing food pellet size. Behav Brain Res 2000;116(2):111-22; Whishaw IQ. Did a change in sensory control of skilled movements stimulate the evolution of the primate frontal cortex? Behav Brain Res 2003;146(1/2):31-41]. We sought to confirm and extend these findings in several ways. In Experiment 1, using a GO/NO-GO variant of the classic task, we demonstrated that rats used the GO target's odor both to trigger and guide their reaches. In Experiment 2, we showed that rats deprived of (a) vision, (b) object-recognizing rostral whiskers and forearm sinus hairs, or (c) both, displayed no deficits in triggering and guiding their reaches. Finally, in a third experiment in which the GO target's location varied randomly across trials and only olfactory cues were available, we demonstrated that rats could determine the spatial endpoint of their reach without any loss of accuracy. Combined with results from a prior study in which bulbectomized rats never developed a new, successful reaching strategy despite extensive post-operative training [Whishaw IQ, Tomie JA. Olfaction directs skilled forelimb reaching in the rat. Behav Brain Res 1989;32(1):11-21], these results indicate that rats do not have a sensory hierarchy for solving the reach-to-grasp-food task, but rather, are guided by olfaction alone until their paw contacts the food morsel.
人类和非人类动物在“选择”解决许多认知和行为任务的策略时会利用感觉层次结构。通常,如果首选类型的感觉信息不可用或对解决给定任务没有用处,动物可以切换到优先级较低的策略,利用不同类别的感觉信息。然而,在大鼠执行经典的伸手抓食物任务的情况下,先前的研究表明,伸手动作可能是一种固定动作模式,在与食物小块接触之前完全由食物的气味羽流引导[Whishaw IQ, Tomie JA. 嗅觉指导大鼠熟练的前肢伸手动作。行为脑科学研究1989;32(1):11 - 21; Metz GA, Whishaw IQ. 熟练伸手是一种动作模式:大鼠(褐家鼠)抓握动作的稳定性与食物颗粒大小变化的关系。行为脑科学研究2000;116(2):111 - 22; Whishaw IQ. 熟练动作的感觉控制变化是否刺激了灵长类前额叶皮层的进化?行为脑科学研究2003;146(1/2):31 - 41]。我们试图通过几种方式来证实并扩展这些发现。在实验1中,使用经典任务的GO/NO - GO变体,我们证明大鼠利用GO目标的气味来触发和引导它们的伸手动作。在实验2中,我们表明剥夺了(a)视觉、(b)识别物体的吻部触须和前臂鼻窦毛发或(c)两者的大鼠,在触发和引导伸手动作方面没有表现出缺陷。最后,在第三个实验中,GO目标的位置在试验中随机变化且只有嗅觉线索可用,我们证明大鼠可以确定它们伸手的空间终点而不会损失任何准确性。结合先前一项研究的结果,即切除嗅球的大鼠尽管经过广泛的术后训练但从未形成新的、成功的伸手策略[Whishaw IQ, Tomie JA. 嗅觉指导大鼠熟练的前肢伸手动作。行为脑科学研究1989;32(1):11 - 21],这些结果表明大鼠在解决伸手抓食物任务时没有感觉层次结构,而是在爪子接触食物小块之前仅由嗅觉引导。