Suppr超能文献

熟悉会滋生轻视:袋鼠会持续避开有实验性投放的澳洲野狗气味的区域。

Familiarity breeds contempt: kangaroos persistently avoid areas with experimentally deployed dingo scents.

机构信息

Centre for Ecosystem Diversity and Dynamics (CEDD), Department of Environmental Biology, Curtin University, Perth, Western Australia, Australia.

出版信息

PLoS One. 2010 May 5;5(5):e10403. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010403.

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Whether or not animals habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents may depend upon whether there are predators associated with the cues. Understanding the contexts of habituation is theoretically important and has profound implication for the application of predator-based herbivore deterrents. We repeatedly exposed a mixed mob of macropod marsupials to olfactory scents (urine, feces) from a sympatric predator (Canis lupus dingo), along with a control (water). If these predator cues were alarming, we expected that over time, some red kangaroos (Macropus rufous), western grey kangaroos (Macropus fuliginosus) and agile wallabies (Macropus agilis) would elect to not participate in cafeteria trials because the scents provided information about the riskiness of the area.

METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We evaluated the effects of urine and feces independently and expected that urine would elicit a stronger reaction because it contains a broader class of infochemicals (pheromones, kairomones). Finally, we scored non-invasive indicators (flight and alarm stomps) to determine whether fear or altered palatability was responsible for the response. Repeated exposure reduced macropodid foraging on food associated with 40 ml of dingo urine, X = 986.75+/-3.97 g food remained as compared to the tap water control, X = 209.0+/-107.0 g (P<0.001). Macropodids fled more when encountering a urine treatment, X = 4.50+/-2.08 flights, as compared to the control, X = 0 flights (P<0.001). There was no difference in effect between urine or feces treatments (P>0.5). Macropodids did not habituate to repeated exposure to predator scents, rather they avoided the entire experimental area after 10 days of trials (R(2) = 83.8; P<0.001).

CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: Responses to urine and feces were indistinguishable; both elicited fear-based responses and deterred foraging. Despite repeated exposure to predator-related cues in the absence of a predator, macropodids persistently avoided an area of highly palatable food. Area avoidance is consistent with that observed from other species following repeated anti-predator conditioning, However, this is the first time this response has been experimentally observed among medium or large vertebrates - where a local response is observed spatially and an area effect is revealed over time.

摘要

背景

动物是否会对重复暴露于捕食者气味产生习惯,可能取决于与线索相关的是否存在捕食者。了解习惯形成的背景在理论上很重要,并且对基于捕食者的食草动物威慑剂的应用具有深远的意义。我们反复将一群混合的有袋目哺乳动物暴露于共生捕食者(Canis lupus dingo)的嗅觉气味(尿液,粪便)中,同时还有对照(水)。如果这些捕食者的气味令人警惕,我们预计随着时间的流逝,一些红袋鼠(Macropus rufous),西部灰袋鼠(Macropus fuliginosus)和敏捷小袋鼠(Macropus agilis)将选择不参加自助餐厅试验,因为这些气味提供了有关该地区风险的信息。

方法/主要发现:我们分别评估了尿液和粪便的作用,并预计尿液会引起更强的反应,因为它包含了更广泛的信息化学物质(信息素,信息素)。最后,我们对非侵入性指标(飞行和警报踩踏)进行了评分,以确定是恐惧还是改变了适口性是造成反应的原因。重复暴露会减少与 40 毫升狼尿相关的有袋动物觅食量,X = 986.75 +/-3.97 g 食物残留量与自来水对照相比,X = 209.0 +/-107.0 g(P <0.001)。与对照相比,遇到尿液处理时,有袋动物的飞行次数更多,X = 4.50 +/-2.08 次,X = 0 次(P <0.001)。尿液或粪便处理之间没有差异(P> 0.5)。有袋动物没有对重复暴露于捕食者气味产生习惯,而是在 10 天的试验后避开了整个实验区域(R(2)= 83.8; P <0.001)。

结论/意义:对尿液和粪便的反应是无法区分的;两者都引起了基于恐惧的反应,并阻止了觅食。尽管在没有捕食者的情况下反复接触与捕食者相关的线索,但有袋动物仍持续避开了一个高度可口的食物区域。区域回避与其他物种在反复进行抗捕食者训练后观察到的情况一致,但是,这是第一次在中型或大型脊椎动物中观察到这种反应-在该反应中,局部反应在空间上是观察到的,并且随着时间的推移,区域效应得到揭示。

https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/77cd/2864734/053f68774f5f/pone.0010403.g001.jpg

相似文献

2
Parasite and predator risk assessment: nuanced use of olfactory cues.
Proc Biol Sci. 2015 Oct 22;282(1817):20151941. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1941.
3
Biologically meaningful scents: a framework for understanding predator-prey research across disciplines.
Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2018 Feb;93(1):98-114. doi: 10.1111/brv.12334. Epub 2017 Apr 26.
5
Olfactory function in the Red Kangaroos (Macropus rufus) assessed using odor-cued taste avoidance.
Physiol Behav. 1999 Sep;67(3):365-8. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00084-0.
6
Fears from the past? The innate ability of dogs to detect predator scents.
Anim Cogn. 2020 Jul;23(4):721-729. doi: 10.1007/s10071-020-01379-y. Epub 2020 Apr 8.
8
Land use and dingo baiting are correlated with the density of kangaroos in rangeland systems.
Integr Zool. 2023 Mar;18(2):299-315. doi: 10.1111/1749-4877.12683. Epub 2022 Oct 12.
9
Experimental evidence of an age-specific shift in chemical detection of predators in a lizard.
J Chem Ecol. 2002 Mar;28(3):541-54. doi: 10.1023/a:1014592028693.

引用本文的文献

2
Odour-mediated Interactions Between an Apex Reptilian Predator and its Mammalian Prey.
J Chem Ecol. 2022 Apr;48(4):401-415. doi: 10.1007/s10886-022-01350-w. Epub 2022 Mar 1.
3
Exploiting common senses: sensory ecology meets wildlife conservation and management.
Conserv Physiol. 2021 Mar 29;9(1):coab002. doi: 10.1093/conphys/coab002. eCollection 2021.
6
Behavioral response of naïve and non-naïve deer to wolf urine.
PLoS One. 2019 Nov 27;14(11):e0223248. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223248. eCollection 2019.
7
The Australian dingo: untamed or feral?
Front Zool. 2019 Feb 13;16:2. doi: 10.1186/s12983-019-0300-6. eCollection 2019.
8
A ghost fence-gap: surprising wildlife usage of an obsolete fence crossing.
PeerJ. 2018 Nov 27;6:e5950. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5950. eCollection 2018.
9
Structure-Activity Relationships of Alkylpyrazine Analogs and Fear-Associated Behaviors in Mice.
J Chem Ecol. 2017 Mar;43(3):263-272. doi: 10.1007/s10886-017-0822-3. Epub 2017 Feb 28.
10
A review of factors influencing the stress response in Australian marsupials.
Conserv Physiol. 2014 Jul 23;2(1):cou027. doi: 10.1093/conphys/cou027. eCollection 2014.

本文引用的文献

2
Sheep food repellents: Efficacy of various products, habituation, and social facilitation.
J Chem Ecol. 1993 Feb;19(2):225-36. doi: 10.1007/BF00993691.
3
Why are predator urines aversive to prey?
J Chem Ecol. 1994 Jul;20(7):1505-16. doi: 10.1007/BF02059876.
4
Mutualism among safe, selfish sentinels: a dynamic game.
Am Nat. 1997 Sep;150(3):373-92. doi: 10.1086/286070.
5
Neural computations with mammalian infochemicals.
J Chem Ecol. 2008 Jul;34(7):928-42. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9483-6. Epub 2008 Jun 14.
7
Identification of putative pheromones in bovine (Bos taurus) faeces in relation to estrus detection.
Anim Reprod Sci. 2008 Jan 15;103(1-2):149-53. doi: 10.1016/j.anireprosci.2007.04.014. Epub 2007 Apr 22.
8
Threat-Sensitive Behavioral Responses to Concentrations of Water-Borne Cues from Predation.
Ethology. 2007 Feb;113(2):199-206. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01317.x.
9
Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment.
Proc Biol Sci. 2005 Dec 22;272(1581):2627-34. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3251.
10
Ferret odor as a processive stress model in rats: neurochemical, behavioral, and endocrine evidence.
Behav Neurosci. 2005 Feb;119(1):280-92. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.280.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验