Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK.
Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK; Institute for Biology, University of Leipzig, Talstraße 33, 04103 Leipzig, Germany.
Curr Biol. 2015 Dec 21;25(24):3245-52. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.064. Epub 2015 Dec 10.
New communication signals can evolve by sensory exploitation if signaling taps into preexisting sensory biases in receivers [1, 2]. For mate attraction, signals are typically similar to attractive environmental cues like food [3-6], which amplifies their attractiveness to mates, as opposed to aversive stimuli like predator cues. Female field crickets approach the low-frequency calling song of males, whereas they avoid high-frequency sounds like predatory bat calls [7]. In one group of crickets (Eneopterinae: Lebinthini), however, males produce exceptionally high-frequency calling songs in the range of bat calls [8], a surprising signal in the context of mate attraction. We found that female lebinthines, instead of approaching singing males, produce vibrational responses after male calls, and males track the source of vibrations to find females. We also demonstrate that field cricket species closely related to the Lebinthini show an acoustic startle response to high-frequency sounds that generates substrate vibrations similar to those produced by female lebinthine crickets. Therefore, the startle response is the most likely evolutionary origin of the female lebinthine vibrational signal. In field crickets, the brain receives activity from two auditory interneurons; AN1 tuned to male calling song controls positive phonotaxis, and AN2 tuned to high-frequency bat calls triggers negative phonotaxis [9, 10]. In lebinthine crickets, however, we found that auditory ascending neurons are only tuned to high-frequency sounds, and their tuning matches the thresholds for female vibrational signals. Our results demonstrate how sensory exploitation of anti-predator behavior can evolve into a communication system that benefits both senders and receivers.
新的通讯信号可以通过感官利用进化而来,如果信号利用了接收者预先存在的感官偏见[1,2]。对于求爱,信号通常与有吸引力的环境线索相似,如食物[3-6],这增加了它们对配偶的吸引力,而不是像捕食者线索那样的厌恶刺激。雌性蟋蟀会接近雄性发出的低频求爱叫声,而避开高频声音,如捕食者的蝙蝠叫声[7]。然而,在一组蟋蟀(Eneopterinae:Lebinthini)中,雄性会发出异常高频的求爱叫声,频率范围与蝙蝠叫声相当[8],这在求爱背景下是一个惊人的信号。我们发现,雌性莱宾氏蟋蟀在雄性鸣叫后会产生振动反应,而雄性则会跟踪振动的来源来寻找雌性。我们还证明,与莱宾氏蟋蟀密切相关的蟋蟀物种对高频声音会产生声学惊跳反应,这种反应会产生类似于雌性莱宾氏蟋蟀产生的基底振动。因此,惊跳反应很可能是雌性莱宾氏蟋蟀振动信号的进化起源。在蟋蟀中,大脑接收来自两个听觉中间神经元的活动;对雄性求爱叫声敏感的 AN1 控制正趋声,而对高频蝙蝠叫声敏感的 AN2 则触发负趋声[9,10]。然而,在莱宾氏蟋蟀中,我们发现听觉上行神经元只对高频声音敏感,它们的调谐与雌性振动信号的阈值相匹配。我们的研究结果表明,如何利用反捕食行为的感官可以进化成一种对发送者和接收者都有益的通讯系统。