University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States; Minnesota Lab for Low Vision Research, 75 E River Rd, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States.
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States; Minnesota Lab for Low Vision Research, 75 E River Rd, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States; HumanFIRST Lab, 111 Church St. SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455, United States.
Exp Eye Res. 2019 Jun;183:20-28. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2018.11.002. Epub 2018 Nov 13.
Navigating unfamiliar indoor spaces while visually searching for objects of interest is a challenge faced by people with visual impairment. We asked how restricting visual acuity of normally sighted subjects would affect visual search and navigation in a real world environment, and how their performance would compare to subjects with naturally occurring low vision. Two experiments were conducted. In the first, 8 normally sighted subjects walked along an indoor path, looking for objects placed at unpredictable intervals to the left and right of the path, and identified single letters posted on the objects. A head-mounted eye tracker was used to assess their gaze direction in the environment. For half the trials, blur foils were used to restrict visual acuity to approximately logMAR 1.65. Gaze behavior, travel time, and letter recognition accuracy were compared between blurred and unrestricted conditions. In the second experiment, the same procedure was conducted, but performance was compared between acuity-restricted normally-sighted subjects and subjects with naturally occurring low vision (mean acuity 1.09 logMAR, range 0.48-1.85 logMAR). In Experiment 1, neither Blur nor the Letter Recognition Task individually had a statistically significant effect on travel time. However, when combined, there was an interaction between the two that increased travel time by approximately 63%, relative to baseline trials. Blur modified gaze behavior such that subjects spent more time looking down toward the floor while walking, at the expense of time spent looking in other directions. During Letter Recognition Task trials with Blur, subjects spent extra time examining objects, though more objects were missed altogether. In Experiment 2, low-vision subjects spent more time looking toward the boundary between the floor and the wall, but gaze patterns were otherwise similar to acuity-restricted subjects with normal vision. Low-vision subjects were also more likely to miss objects compared to acuity-restricted subjects. We conclude that under conditions of artificially restricted acuity, normally sighted subjects look downward toward the floor more frequently while navigating and take extra time to examine objects of interest, but are less likely to detect them. Low-vision subjects tend to direct their gaze toward the boundary between the wall and the floor, which may serve as a high contrast cue for navigation.
在视觉搜索感兴趣的物体时,在不熟悉的室内空间中导航对视力受损的人来说是一个挑战。我们想知道限制正常视力者的视力敏锐度会如何影响他们在真实环境中的视觉搜索和导航,以及他们的表现与自然发生低视力者相比如何。进行了两项实验。在第一项实验中,8 名正常视力的受试者沿着室内路径行走,在路径的左右两侧寻找放置在不可预测间隔处的物体,并识别贴在物体上的单个字母。使用头戴式眼动追踪器评估他们在环境中的注视方向。在一半的试验中,使用模糊箔片将视力限制在大约 logMAR 1.65。比较了模糊和无限制条件下的注视行为、旅行时间和字母识别准确性。在第二项实验中,进行了相同的程序,但将视力受限的正常视力受试者和自然发生低视力受试者(平均视力 1.09 logMAR,范围 0.48-1.85 logMAR)的表现进行了比较。在实验 1 中,Blur 和 Letter Recognition Task 都没有单独对旅行时间产生统计学上显著的影响。然而,当两者结合时,它们之间存在相互作用,使旅行时间相对于基线试验增加了约 63%。Blur 改变了注视行为,使受试者在行走时更多地向下看地板,而减少了向其他方向看的时间。在带有 Blur 的 Letter Recognition Task 试验中,受试者花费额外的时间检查物体,尽管更多的物体完全被忽略了。在实验 2 中,低视力受试者更多地看向地板和墙壁之间的边界,但注视模式与视力受限的正常视力受试者基本相似。与视力受限的正常视力受试者相比,低视力受试者更容易错过物体。我们得出的结论是,在人为限制视力的情况下,正常视力的受试者在导航时更频繁地向下看向地板,并花费额外的时间检查感兴趣的物体,但不太可能发现它们。低视力受试者倾向于将目光投向墙壁和地板之间的边界,这可能是导航的高对比度线索。