Department of Human Biology, University of Wrocław, ul. Przybyszewskiego 63, 51-148, Wrocław, Poland.
Theory Biosci. 2023 Sep;142(3):199-203. doi: 10.1007/s12064-023-00393-1. Epub 2023 Jun 5.
Adaptive mimicry in animals is a well-known phenomenon. Here, we propose that a similarly adaptive strategy in humans is using kin terms for people who are not closely genetically related. Irrespective of the initiator attributing a kin term to a non-kin, we call this kin term mimicry (KTM). The emergence of human sociality and language allowed not only easy kin recognition, but also led to strong positive emotions related to such kin names as "mother," "father," "brother," "sister," "aunt" or "uncle." Although the phenomenon of using kin terms of genetically unrelated people is well known in the social sciences, here we discuss it in the light of evolution. We notice this is an evolutionary adaptive cooperation strategy, which allows us to predict in which ecological or social circumstances it will be more prevalent. We postulate specific testable factors that affect the prevalence of kin mimicry. We also discuss who is more likely to be an initiator of calling non-kin a fictive kin, and who benefits from such behavior. The KTM hypothesis postulates that an individual or social group initiating or bestowing kin terms usually receives more benefits (economic and/or psychological support) from such mimicry.
动物的适应模仿是一种众所周知的现象。在这里,我们提出人类也有一种类似的适应性策略,即用亲属称谓来称呼那些与自己没有密切遗传关系的人。无论发起者是否将亲属称谓赋予非亲属,我们都称之为亲属称谓模仿(KTM)。人类社会和语言的出现不仅使亲属识别变得容易,还导致了与“母亲”、“父亲”、“兄弟”、“姐妹”、“阿姨”或“叔叔”等亲属名称相关的强烈积极情绪。虽然在社会科学中,用与遗传上无关的人的亲属称谓的现象是众所周知的,但在这里,我们从进化的角度来讨论它。我们注意到这是一种进化适应性的合作策略,这使我们能够预测在哪些生态或社会环境中它会更普遍。我们提出了影响亲属模仿流行程度的具体可测试因素。我们还讨论了谁更有可能成为称呼非亲属为虚构亲属的发起者,以及谁从这种行为中受益。KTM 假说假设,发起或赋予亲属称谓的个体或社会群体通常会从这种模仿中获得更多的利益(经济和/或心理支持)。