Suppr超能文献

大鼠通过可变间隔强化按压杠杆的反应发作。

Bouts of responding from variable-interval reinforcement of lever pressing by rats.

作者信息

Shull Richard L, Grimes Julie A

机构信息

Department of Psychology, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 27402-6170, USA.

出版信息

J Exp Anal Behav. 2003 Sep;80(2):159-71. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2003.80-159.

Abstract

Four rats obtained food pellets by lever pressing. A variable-interval reinforcement schedule assigned reinforcers on average every 2 min during one block of 20 sessions and on average every 8 min during another block. Also, at each variable-interval duration, a block of sessions was conducted with a schedule that imposed a variable-ratio 4 response requirement after each variable interval (i.e., a tandem variable-time variable-ratio 4 schedule). The total rate of lever pressing increased as a function of the rate of reinforcement and as a result of imposing the variable-ratio requirement. Analysis of log survivor plots of interresponse times indicated that lever pressing occurred in bouts that were separated by pauses. Increasing the rate of reinforcement increased total response rate by increasing the rate of initiating bouts and, less reliably, by lengthening bouts. Imposing the variable-ratio component increased response rate mainly by lengthening bouts. This pattern of results is similar to that reported previously with key poking as the response. Also, response rates within bouts were relatively insensitive to either variable.

摘要

四只大鼠通过按压杠杆获取食物颗粒。在一个由20次实验组成的阶段中,可变间隔强化程序平均每2分钟给予一次强化,在另一个阶段则平均每8分钟给予一次强化。此外,在每个可变间隔时长,会进行一个实验阶段,该阶段在每个可变间隔后施加一个4次反应的可变比率要求(即串联可变时间可变比率4程序)。杠杆按压的总速率随着强化速率的增加以及由于施加可变比率要求而增加。对反应间隔时间的对数存活曲线分析表明,杠杆按压是以有停顿间隔的阵发形式出现的。增加强化速率通过提高阵发起始速率来提高总反应速率,不太可靠的是通过延长阵发时间。施加可变比率成分主要通过延长阵发时间来提高反应速率。这种结果模式与之前以按键作为反应所报道的结果相似。此外,阵发内的反应速率对这两个变量都相对不敏感。

相似文献

1
Bouts of responding from variable-interval reinforcement of lever pressing by rats.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2003 Sep;80(2):159-71. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2003.80-159.
2
Bouts of responding: the relation between bout rate and the rate of variable-interval reinforcement.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2004 Jan;81(1):65-83. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2004.81-65.
3
Bouts of responding on variable-interval schedules: effects of deprivation level.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2004 Mar;81(2):155-67. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2004.81-155.
4
Response rate viewed as engagement bouts: effects of relative reinforcement and schedule type.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2001 May;75(3):247-74. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-247.
5
The distribution of response bout lengths and its sensitivity to differential reinforcement.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2015 Sep;104(2):167-85. doi: 10.1002/jeab.168.
6
Automatic reinforcement from operant wheel-running undermines temporal control by fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement.
Behav Processes. 2018 Dec;157:91-101. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.09.003. Epub 2018 Sep 13.
8
Discrimination of variable schedules is controlled by interresponse times proximal to reinforcement.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2012 Nov;98(3):341-54. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2012.98-341.
9
Techniques for establishing schedules with wheel running as reinforcement in rats.
J Exp Anal Behav. 1993 Jul;60(1):219-38. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.60-219.

引用本文的文献

1
YEAB: YEAB Ease the analysis of behavior.
Behav Res Methods. 2025 Jun 16;57(7):199. doi: 10.3758/s13428-025-02720-4.
2
Bouts, Pauses, and Units of Operant Performance: A Primer.
Perspect Behav Sci. 2024 Aug 30;47(3):643-674. doi: 10.1007/s40614-024-00419-z. eCollection 2024 Sep.
3
The temporal structure of goal-directed and habitual operant behavior.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2024 Jan;121(1):38-51. doi: 10.1002/jeab.896. Epub 2023 Dec 22.
4
Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex activation disrupts Pavlovian incentive motivation.
Front Behav Neurosci. 2022 Oct 13;16:999320. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.999320. eCollection 2022.
5
Longer operant lever-press duration requirements induce fewer but longer response bouts in rats.
Learn Behav. 2021 Sep;49(3):330-342. doi: 10.3758/s13420-021-00464-7. Epub 2021 Feb 24.
6
A computational formulation of the behavior systems account of the temporal organization of motivated behavior.
Behav Processes. 2019 Dec;169:103952. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103952. Epub 2019 Sep 20.
7
Suppressive and enhancing effects of nicotine on food-seeking behavior.
Behav Brain Res. 2018 Feb 26;339:130-139. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.11.023. Epub 2017 Nov 23.
8
A bout analysis of operant response disruption.
Behav Processes. 2017 Aug;141(Pt 1):42-49. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.04.008. Epub 2017 Apr 19.

本文引用的文献

2
Constituents of response rates.
J Exp Anal Behav. 1979 Nov;32(3):341-62. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1979.32-341.
3
The effect of reinforcement magnitude upon responding under fixed-ratio schedules.
J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Jul;12(4):605-8. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1969.12-605.
4
Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: a new method and some data.
J Exp Anal Behav. 1963 Apr;6(2):237-46. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1963.6-237.
5
The rats rate of drinking as a function of water deprivation.
J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1952 Feb;45(1):96-102. doi: 10.1037/h0062150.
6
Punishment and recovery during fixed-ratio performance.
J Exp Anal Behav. 1959 Oct;2(4):301-5. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1959.2-301.
7
Fundamental dimensional properties of the operant.
Psychol Rev. 1958 Sep;65(5):272-82. doi: 10.1037/h0044071.
8
Satiation effects under fixed-ratio schedules of reinforcement.
J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954 Apr;47(2):114-6. doi: 10.1037/h0054127.
9
Running time as a function of amount of food deprivation.
J Exp Psychol. 1953 Sep;46(3):188-98. doi: 10.1037/h0061014.
10
Molecular analyses of the principal components of response strength.
J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Sep;78(2):127-60. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-127.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验