Suppr超能文献

群居食肉动物中的“讨厌邻居”而非“可敬的敌人”

'Nasty neighbours' rather than 'dear enemies' in a social carnivore.

作者信息

Müller Corsin A, Manser Marta B

机构信息

Department of Animal Behaviour, Institute of Zoology, University of Zürich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland.

出版信息

Proc Biol Sci. 2007 Apr 7;274(1612):959-65. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0222.

Abstract

Territorial animals typically respond less aggressively to neighbours than to strangers. This 'dear enemy effect' has been explained by differing familiarity or by different threat levels posed by neighbours and strangers. In most species, both the familiarity and the threat-level hypotheses predict a stronger response to strangers than to neighbours. In contrast, the threat-level hypothesis predicts a stronger response to neighbours than to strangers in species with intense competition between neighbours and with residents outnumbering strangers, as commonly found in social mammals such as the banded mongoose (Mungos mungo). The familiarity hypothesis predicts reduced aggression towards neighbours also in these species. We exposed free-living banded mongoose groups to translocated scent marks of neighbouring groups and strangers. Groups vocalized more and inspected more samples in response to olfactory cues of the neighbours than to the strangers. Our results support the threat-level hypothesis and contradict the familiarity hypothesis. We suggest that increased aggression towards neighbours is more common in social species with intense competition between neighbours, as opposed to reduced aggression towards neighbours typical for most solitary species.

摘要

领地动物通常对邻居的攻击性比对陌生人的攻击性要小。这种“亲爱的敌人效应”已通过不同程度的熟悉度或邻居和陌生人所带来的不同威胁程度来解释。在大多数物种中,熟悉度假说和威胁程度假说都预测对陌生人的反应比对邻居的反应更强烈。相比之下,威胁程度假说预测,在邻居之间竞争激烈且居民数量超过陌生人的物种中,对邻居的反应比对陌生人的反应更强烈,在诸如带状猫鼬(白尾獴)这样的社会性哺乳动物中通常就是如此。熟悉度假说也预测在这些物种中对邻居的攻击性会降低。我们将自由生活的带状猫鼬群体暴露于相邻群体和陌生人的转移气味标记中。与陌生人相比,群体对邻居的嗅觉线索发出更多叫声并检查更多样本。我们的结果支持威胁程度假说,与熟悉度假说相矛盾。我们认为,在邻居之间竞争激烈的社会性物种中,对邻居增加攻击性更为常见,这与大多数独居物种对邻居攻击性降低的情况相反。

相似文献

1
'Nasty neighbours' rather than 'dear enemies' in a social carnivore.
Proc Biol Sci. 2007 Apr 7;274(1612):959-65. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0222.
2
Context-dependent responses to neighbours and strangers in wild European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus).
Behav Processes. 2014 Jul;106:17-21. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.04.004. Epub 2014 Apr 18.
3
Are unfamiliar neighbours considered to be dear-enemies?
PLoS One. 2010 Aug 26;5(8):e12428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012428.
4
Modulation of dear enemy effects by male dusky warblers (Phylloscopus fuscatus) at different reproductive stages.
Behav Processes. 2022 Aug;200:104706. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104706. Epub 2022 Jul 14.
6
7
How to coexist with fire ants: the roles of behaviour and cuticular compounds.
Behav Processes. 2013 Sep;98:51-7. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.04.014. Epub 2013 Apr 30.
8
Dear enemy effect in the Mexican volcano mouse Neotomodon alstoni: Implications of sex in the agonistic behaviour among neighbours.
Behav Processes. 2020 Dec;181:104251. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104251. Epub 2020 Sep 22.
9
Male song sparrows have elevated testosterone in response to neighbors versus strangers.
Horm Behav. 2017 Jul;93:47-52. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.04.006. Epub 2017 May 12.
10

引用本文的文献

1
Feeling Disconnected: River Fragmentation Alters Parenting, Aggression, and Risk-Taking in Threespine Stickleback.
Ecol Evol. 2025 Sep 15;15(9):e72099. doi: 10.1002/ece3.72099. eCollection 2025 Sep.
2
Familiarity mediated by body size predicts intraspecific aggression in farming damselfishes.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2025;79(9):89. doi: 10.1007/s00265-025-03636-x. Epub 2025 Aug 30.
3
To Hunt or Patrol? Social Composition and Location Mediate Scent Marking Decisions of a Large Carnivore.
Ecol Evol. 2025 Jun 18;15(6):e71567. doi: 10.1002/ece3.71567. eCollection 2025 Jun.
5
The nasty neighbor effect in humans.
Sci Adv. 2024 Jun 28;10(26):eadm7968. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.adm7968. Epub 2024 Jun 26.
6
Size matters: Natural experiments suggest the dear enemy effect is moderated by pack size in African wild dogs.
Ecol Evol. 2024 Apr 17;14(4):e11298. doi: 10.1002/ece3.11298. eCollection 2024 Apr.
7
The dynamics of social cohesion in response to simulated intergroup conflict in banded mongooses.
Ecol Evol. 2021 Dec 20;11(24):18662-18675. doi: 10.1002/ece3.8475. eCollection 2021 Dec.
8
Kinship, dear enemies, and costly combat: The effects of relatedness on territorial overlap and aggression in a cooperative breeder.
Ecol Evol. 2021 Nov 19;11(23):17031-17042. doi: 10.1002/ece3.8342. eCollection 2021 Dec.
9
Sexual Dimorphism in Aggression: Sex-Specific Fighting Strategies Across Species.
Front Behav Neurosci. 2021 Jun 28;15:659615. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.659615. eCollection 2021.
10
The dear enemy effect drives conspecific aggressiveness in an Azteca-Cecropia system.
Sci Rep. 2021 Mar 17;11(1):6158. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-85070-3.

本文引用的文献

1
Faecal glucocorticoid metabolites and alarm calling in free-living yellow-bellied marmots.
Biol Lett. 2006 Mar 22;2(1):29-32. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0405.
3
Male red-winged blackbirds distrust unreliable and sexually attractive neighbours.
Proc Biol Sci. 2004 May 22;271(1543):1033-8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2687.
4
Individual voice recognition in a territorial frog (Rana catesbeiana).
Proc Biol Sci. 2002 Jul 22;269(1499):1443-8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2041.
5
Social control of reproduction in banded mongooses.
Anim Behav. 2000 Jan;59(1):147-158. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1279.
6
Conflict and assessment in a predator-prey system: ground squirrels versus rattlesnakes.
Anim Behav. 1999 May;57(5):1033-1044. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.1069.
7
Responses of aardwolves, Proteles cristatus, Sparrman 1783, to translocated scent marks.
Anim Behav. 1998 Jul;56(1):137-46. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0757.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验