Suppr超能文献

味觉和嗅觉信号在风味感知中的共激活。

Coactivation of gustatory and olfactory signals in flavor perception.

机构信息

The John B Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

出版信息

Chem Senses. 2010 Feb;35(2):121-33. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjp089. Epub 2009 Dec 23.

Abstract

It is easier to detect mixtures of gustatory and olfactory flavorants than to detect either component alone. But does the detection of mixtures exceed the level predicted by probability summation, assuming independent detection of each component? To answer this question, we measured simple response times (RTs) to detect brief pulses of one of 3 flavorants (sucrose [gustatory], citral [olfactory], sucrose-citral mixture) or water, presented into the mouth by a computer-operated, automated flow system. Subjects were instructed to press a button as soon as they detected any of the 3 nonwater stimuli. Responses to the mixtures were faster (RTs smaller) than predicted by a model of probability summation of independently detected signals, suggesting positive coactivation (integration) of gustation and retronasal olfaction in flavor perception. Evidence for integration appeared mainly in the fastest 60% of the responses, indicating that integration arises relatively early in flavor processing. Results were similar when the 3 possible flavorants, and water, were interleaved within the same session (experimental condition), and when each flavorant was interleaved with water only (control conditions). This outcome suggests that subjects did not attend selectively to one flavor component or the other in the experimental condition and further supports the conclusion that (late) decisional or attentional strategies do not exert a large influence on the gustatory-olfactory flavor integration.

摘要

与单独检测一种味觉或嗅觉成分相比,人们更容易检测到味觉和嗅觉成分的混合物。但是,假设对每个成分进行独立检测,混合物的检测是否超过概率总和的水平?为了回答这个问题,我们使用计算机操作的自动流系统将 3 种味道剂(蔗糖[味觉]、柠檬醛[嗅觉]、蔗糖-柠檬醛混合物)或水之一短暂脉冲传递到口中,测量了简单的反应时间(RT)以检测到这些刺激中的任何一种。要求被试者一旦检测到任何 3 种非水刺激物之一,就立即按下按钮。与独立检测信号的概率总和模型的预测相比,对混合物的反应更快(RT 更小),这表明味觉和鼻后嗅觉在味道感知中存在正协同激活(整合)。整合的证据主要出现在最快的 60%的反应中,这表明整合在味道处理中相对较早出现。当 3 种可能的味道剂和水在同一会话(实验条件)中混合,或者当每种味道剂仅与水混合(对照条件)时,结果相似。这一结果表明,被试者在实验条件下并没有选择性地关注一种味道成分或另一种味道成分,进一步支持了这样的结论,即(后期)决策或注意力策略对味觉-嗅觉味道整合没有很大影响。

相似文献

1
Coactivation of gustatory and olfactory signals in flavor perception.
Chem Senses. 2010 Feb;35(2):121-33. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjp089. Epub 2009 Dec 23.
2
Response Times to Gustatory-Olfactory Flavor Mixtures: Role of Congruence.
Chem Senses. 2015 Oct;40(8):565-75. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv042. Epub 2015 Aug 24.
3
Flavor-intensity perception: effects of stimulus context.
Physiol Behav. 2012 Jan 18;105(2):443-50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.039. Epub 2011 Sep 10.
4
Processing of Intraoral Olfactory and Gustatory Signals in the Gustatory Cortex of Awake Rats.
J Neurosci. 2017 Jan 11;37(2):244-257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1926-16.2016.
5
Identification of gustatory-olfactory flavor mixtures: effects of linguistic labeling.
Chem Senses. 2013 May;38(4):305-13. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjs142. Epub 2013 Jan 16.
6
Detecting gustatory-olfactory flavor mixtures: models of probability summation.
Chem Senses. 2012 Mar;37(3):263-77. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjr103. Epub 2011 Nov 10.
8
Single-neuron responses to intraoral delivery of odor solutions in primary olfactory and gustatory cortex.
J Neurophysiol. 2017 Mar 1;117(3):1293-1304. doi: 10.1152/jn.00802.2016. Epub 2016 Dec 21.
9
Flavor Identification and Intensity: Effects of Stimulus Context.
Chem Senses. 2016 Mar;41(3):249-59. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv087. Epub 2016 Jan 31.
10
Interactions of Lemon, Sucrose and Citric Acid in Enhancing Citrus, Sweet and Sour Flavors.
Chem Senses. 2017 Dec 25;43(1):17-26. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjx063.

引用本文的文献

1
Taste enhances the ability to express a preference for a congruent odor in rats.
Behav Neurosci. 2024 Dec;138(6):433-440. doi: 10.1037/bne0000605. Epub 2024 Sep 19.
3
The effect of multisensory context and experience on flavor preference decisions in rats.
Physiol Behav. 2024 Mar 15;276:114480. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2024.114480. Epub 2024 Feb 1.
6
Noise-rearing precludes the behavioral benefits of multisensory integration.
Cereb Cortex. 2023 Feb 7;33(4):948-958. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac113.
7
Food reinforcement and habituation to food are processes related to initiation and cessation of eating.
Physiol Behav. 2021 Oct 1;239:113512. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113512. Epub 2021 Jul 1.
8
Strawberry sweetness and consumer preference are enhanced by specific volatile compounds.
Hortic Res. 2021 Apr 1;8(1):66. doi: 10.1038/s41438-021-00502-5.
9
Adaptive weighting of taste and odor cues during flavor choice.
J Neurophysiol. 2020 Dec 1;124(6):1942-1947. doi: 10.1152/jn.00506.2020. Epub 2020 Oct 7.
10
Identification of an Amygdala-Thalamic Circuit That Acts as a Central Gain Mechanism in Taste Perceptions.
J Neurosci. 2020 Jun 24;40(26):5051-5062. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2618-19.2020. Epub 2020 May 5.

本文引用的文献

2
The multisensory perception of flavor.
Conscious Cogn. 2008 Sep;17(3):1016-31. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.06.005. Epub 2007 Aug 3.
3
Temporal synchrony and integration of sub-threshold taste and smell signals.
Chem Senses. 2005 Sep;30(7):539-45. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bji047. Epub 2005 Jul 27.
4
Comparison times are longer for hedonic than for intensity judgements of taste stimuli.
Physiol Behav. 2005 Mar 16;84(3):489-95. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.01.018.
5
Effect of endogenous attention on detection of weak gustatory and olfactory flavors.
Percept Psychophys. 2004 May;66(4):596-608. doi: 10.3758/bf03194904.
6
The Temporal Automated System for Taste Experiments (TASTE).
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004 Feb;36(1):83-8. doi: 10.3758/bf03195552.
7
Validation of a stimulation protocol suited to the investigation of odor-taste interactions with fMRI.
Physiol Behav. 2004 May;81(3):389-96. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.12.018.
8
A new method for comparing orthonasal and retronasal olfaction.
Behav Neurosci. 2004 Apr;118(2):412-9. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.412.
9
Experience-dependent neural integration of taste and smell in the human brain.
J Neurophysiol. 2004 Sep;92(3):1892-903. doi: 10.1152/jn.00050.2004. Epub 2004 Apr 21.
10
The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration.
Curr Biol. 2004 Feb 3;14(3):257-62. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验