Suppr超能文献

传递性推理中的认知表征:四种鸦科鸟类的比较

Cognitive representation in transitive inference: a comparison of four corvid species.

作者信息

Bond Alan B, Wei Cynthia A, Kamil Alan C

机构信息

School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska, 348 Manter Hall, Lincoln, NE 68588-0118, USA.

出版信息

Behav Processes. 2010 Oct;85(3):283-92. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.08.003. Epub 2010 Aug 11.

Abstract

During operant transitive inference experiments, subjects are trained on adjacent stimulus pairs in an implicit linear hierarchy in which responses to higher ranked stimuli are rewarded. Two contrasting forms of cognitive representation are often used to explain resulting choice behavior. Associative representation is based on memory for the reward history of each stimulus. Relational representation depends on memory for the context in which stimuli have been presented. Natural history characteristics that require accurate configural memory, such as social complexity or reliance on cached food, should tend to promote greater use of relational representation. To test this hypothesis, four corvid species with contrasting natural histories were trained on the transitive inference task: pinyon jays, Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus; Clark's nutcrackers, Nucifraga columbiana; azure-winged magpies, Cyanopica cyanus; and western scrub jays, Aphelocoma californica. A simplified computer model of associative representation displayed a characteristic pattern of accuracy as a function of position in the hierarchy. Analysis of the deviation of each subject's performance from this predicted pattern yielded an index of reliance on relational representation. Regression of index scores against rankings of social complexity and caching reliance indicated that both traits were significantly and independently associated with greater use of relational representation.

摘要

在操作性传递推理实验中,受试者在一个隐含的线性层级结构中接受相邻刺激对的训练,其中对较高等级刺激的反应会得到奖励。两种截然不同的认知表征形式常被用来解释由此产生的选择行为。联想表征基于对每个刺激奖励历史的记忆。关系表征则依赖于对呈现刺激的情境的记忆。需要精确构型记忆的自然历史特征,如社会复杂性或对储存食物的依赖,应该倾向于促进对关系表征的更多使用。为了验证这一假设,对四种具有不同自然历史的鸦科鸟类进行了传递推理任务训练:美洲矮松鸦(Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus)、克拉克星鸦(Nucifraga columbiana)、蓝翅喜鹊(Cyanopica cyanus)和西丛鸦(Aphelocoma californica)。一个简化的联想表征计算机模型显示了一种作为层级结构中位置函数的特征性准确率模式。分析每个受试者的表现与该预测模式的偏差,得出了对关系表征依赖程度的指标。将指标分数与社会复杂性和储存依赖程度的排名进行回归分析表明,这两个特征都与更多地使用关系表征显著且独立相关。

相似文献

1
Cognitive representation in transitive inference: a comparison of four corvid species.
Behav Processes. 2010 Oct;85(3):283-92. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.08.003. Epub 2010 Aug 11.
4
Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog.
J Comp Psychol. 1994 Dec;108(4):385-93. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.4.385.
6
A comparison of four corvid species in a working and reference memory task using a radial maze.
J Comp Psychol. 2000 Dec;114(4):347-56. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.4.347.
8
Direct and relational representation during transitive list linking in pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus).
J Comp Psychol. 2014 Feb;128(1):1-10. doi: 10.1037/a0034627. Epub 2013 Nov 4.
9
Differences in hippocampal volume among food storing corvids.
Brain Behav Evol. 1996;47(3):156-64. doi: 10.1159/000113235.
10
Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) are sensitive to distance, but not lighting when caching in the presence of a conspecific.
Behav Processes. 2016 Feb;123:125-33. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.10.023. Epub 2015 Nov 12.

引用本文的文献

1
Transitive reasoning in the adult domestic hen in a six-term series task.
Anim Cogn. 2024 Nov 19;27(1):77. doi: 10.1007/s10071-024-01914-1.
2
Effects of posttransfer feedback informativeness in a transitive inference task.
Mem Cognit. 2025 Jul;53(5):1330-1342. doi: 10.3758/s13421-024-01654-0. Epub 2024 Oct 30.
3
Transitive inference as probabilistic preference learning.
Psychon Bull Rev. 2025 Apr;32(2):674-689. doi: 10.3758/s13423-024-02600-6. Epub 2024 Oct 22.
4
Transitive inference in a clinical childhood sample with a focus on autism spectrum disorder.
Autism Res. 2024 Nov;17(11):2355-2369. doi: 10.1002/aur.3225. Epub 2024 Sep 2.
5
In the absence of extensive initial training, cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus fail a transitive inference task.
PLoS One. 2023 Jun 23;18(6):e0287402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0287402. eCollection 2023.
7
Heuristics Facilitates the Evolution of Transitive Inference and Social Hierarchy in a Large Group.
Acta Biotheor. 2023 Mar 3;71(2):8. doi: 10.1007/s10441-023-09459-5.
8
Transitive inference in cleaner wrasses (Labroides dimidiatus).
PLoS One. 2020 Aug 18;15(8):e0237817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237817. eCollection 2020.

本文引用的文献

1
Transitive responding in animals and humans: Exaptation rather than adaptation?
Behav Processes. 1998 Feb;42(2-3):107-37. doi: 10.1016/s0376-6357(97)00072-7.
2
Transitive responding in pigeons: influences of stimulus frequency and reinforcement history.
Behav Processes. 1996 Sep;37(2-3):185-95. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(96)00020-4.
3
The role of the hippocampus in transitive inference.
Psychiatry Res. 2009 Apr 30;172(1):24-30. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.008. Epub 2009 Feb 12.
4
Cognitive maps in rats and men.
Psychol Rev. 1948 Jul;55(4):189-208. doi: 10.1037/h0061626.
5
Transitive inference in non-human animals: an empirical and theoretical analysis.
Behav Processes. 2008 Jul;78(3):313-34. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.017. Epub 2008 Mar 7.
7
Fish can infer social rank by observation alone.
Nature. 2007 Jan 25;445(7126):429-32. doi: 10.1038/nature05511.
8
From movement to transitivity: the role of hippocampal parallel maps in configural learning.
Rev Neurosci. 2006;17(1-2):99-109. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2006.17.1-2.99.
9
An evolutionary perspective on caching by corvids.
Proc Biol Sci. 2006 Feb 22;273(1585):417-23. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3350.
10
Effect of stimulus orderability and reinforcement history on transitive responding in pigeons.
Behav Processes. 2006 May 1;72(2):161-72. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.01.008. Epub 2006 Feb 7.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验