Pegado Felipe, Comerlato Enio, Ventura Fabricio, Jobert Antoinette, Nakamura Kimihiro, Buiatti Marco, Ventura Paulo, Dehaene-Lambertz Ghislaine, Kolinsky Régine, Morais José, Braga Lucia W, Cohen Laurent, Dehaene Stanislas
Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, 91191 Gif sur Yvette, France; Neurospin Center, Commissariat à l'énergie atomique (CEA), Division Sciences de la Vie (DSV), Institut d'imagerie Biomédicale (I2BM), 91191 Gif sur Yvette, France; University Paris 11, 91405 Orsay, France; Laboratory of Biological Psychology, KU Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium;
SARAH Network, International Center for Neurosciences and Rehabilitation, 71.535-005 Brasilia, Brazil;
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014 Dec 9;111(49):E5233-42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417347111. Epub 2014 Nov 24.
Learning to read requires the acquisition of an efficient visual procedure for quickly recognizing fine print. Thus, reading practice could induce a perceptual learning effect in early vision. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in literate and illiterate adults, we previously demonstrated an impact of reading acquisition on both high- and low-level occipitotemporal visual areas, but could not resolve the time course of these effects. To clarify whether literacy affects early vs. late stages of visual processing, we measured event-related potentials to various categories of visual stimuli in healthy adults with variable levels of literacy, including completely illiterate subjects, early-schooled literate subjects, and subjects who learned to read in adulthood (ex-illiterates). The stimuli included written letter strings forming pseudowords, on which literacy is expected to have a major impact, as well as faces, houses, tools, checkerboards, and false fonts. To evaluate the precision with which these stimuli were encoded, we studied repetition effects by presenting the stimuli in pairs composed of repeated, mirrored, or unrelated pictures from the same category. The results indicate that reading ability is correlated with a broad enhancement of early visual processing, including increased repetition suppression, suggesting better exemplar discrimination, and increased mirror discrimination, as early as ∼ 100-150 ms in the left occipitotemporal region. These effects were found with letter strings and false fonts, but also were partially generalized to other visual categories. Thus, learning to read affects the magnitude, precision, and invariance of early visual processing.
学习阅读需要掌握一种高效的视觉程序,以便快速识别小号字体。因此,阅读练习可能会在早期视觉中引发知觉学习效应。我们之前利用功能磁共振成像(fMRI)对识字和不识字的成年人进行研究,证明了阅读习得对枕颞叶高低层次视觉区域均有影响,但无法确定这些影响的时间进程。为了阐明识字是否会影响视觉处理的早期阶段与后期阶段,我们对识字水平各异的健康成年人(包括完全不识字的受试者、接受过早期教育的识字受试者以及成年后才学会阅读的受试者(曾经的文盲))进行了测量,记录他们对各类视觉刺激的事件相关电位。刺激物包括组成假词的书面字母串(预计识字对其有重大影响)以及面孔、房屋、工具、棋盘和伪字体。为了评估对这些刺激进行编码的精度,我们通过将刺激物呈现为由同一类别的重复、镜像或不相关图片组成的对子来研究重复效应。结果表明,阅读能力与早期视觉处理的广泛增强相关,包括重复抑制增加(表明对范例的辨别能力更强)以及镜像辨别增加,最早在左侧枕颞叶区域约100 - 150毫秒时就出现这些情况。这些效应在字母串和伪字体中被发现,而且在一定程度上也推广到了其他视觉类别。因此,学习阅读会影响早期视觉处理的程度、精度和不变性。