Neggers S F, Bekkering H
Department of Psychonomics, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.
J Neurophysiol. 2001 Aug;86(2):961-70. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.961.
A well-coordinated pattern of eye and hand movements can be observed during goal-directed arm movements. Typically, a saccadic eye movement precedes the arm movement, and its occurrence is temporally correlated with the start of the arm movement. Furthermore, the coupling of gaze and aiming movements is also observable after pointing initiation. It has recently been observed that saccades cannot be directed to new target stimuli, away from a pointing target stimulus. Saccades directed to targets presented during the final phase of a pointing movement were delayed until after pointing movement offset ("gaze anchoring"). The present study investigated whether ocular gaze is anchored to a pointing target during the entire pointing movement. In experiment 1, new targets were presented at various times during the duration of a pointing movement, triggered by the kinematics arm moment itself (movement onset, peak acceleration/velocity/deceleration, and offset). Subjects had to make a saccade to the new target as fast as possible while maintaining the pointing movement to the initial target. Saccadic latencies were increased by an amount of time that approximately equaled the remaining pointing time after saccadic target presentation, with the majority of saccades executed after pointing movement offset. The nature of the signal driving gaze stabilization during pointing was investigated in experiment 2. In previous experiments where ocular gaze was anchored to a pointing target, subjects could always see their moving arm, thus it was unknown whether a visual image of the moving arm, an afferent (proprioceptive) signal or an efferent (motor control related) signal produced gaze anchoring. In experiment 2 subjects had to point with or without vision of the moving arm to test whether a visual signal is used to anchor gaze to a pointing target. Results indicate that gaze anchoring was also observed without vision of the moving arm. The findings support the existence of a mechanism enforcing ocular gaze anchoring during the entire duration of a pointing movement. Moreover, such a mechanism uses an internally generated, or proprioceptive, nonvisual signal. Possible neural substrates underlying these processes are discussed, as well as the role of selective attention.
在目标导向的手臂运动过程中,可以观察到一种眼手运动协调良好的模式。通常,扫视眼动先于手臂运动,其发生与手臂运动的开始在时间上相关。此外,在指向动作开始后,注视和瞄准动作的耦合也可观察到。最近有人观察到,扫视不能被引导至远离指向目标刺激的新目标刺激。指向运动最后阶段呈现的目标的扫视会延迟,直到指向运动结束(“注视锚定”)。本研究调查了在整个指向运动过程中,眼注视是否锚定在指向目标上。在实验1中,在指向运动持续期间的不同时间呈现新目标,由手臂运动的运动学参数本身(运动开始、峰值加速度/速度/减速度和结束)触发。受试者必须在保持向初始目标的指向运动的同时,尽快对新目标进行扫视。扫视潜伏期增加的时间量大约等于扫视目标呈现后剩余的指向时间,大多数扫视在指向运动结束后执行。实验2研究了指向过程中驱动注视稳定的信号的性质。在先前的实验中,眼注视被锚定在指向目标上,受试者总能看到自己移动的手臂,因此尚不清楚是移动手臂的视觉图像、传入(本体感觉)信号还是传出(运动控制相关)信号产生了注视锚定。在实验2中,受试者在有或没有看到移动手臂的情况下进行指向,以测试是否使用视觉信号将注视锚定在指向目标上。结果表明,在没有看到移动手臂的情况下也观察到了注视锚定。这些发现支持了在指向运动的整个持续时间内存在一种强制眼注视锚定的机制。此外,这样一种机制使用内部产生的或本体感觉的非视觉信号。讨论了这些过程潜在的可能神经基础,以及选择性注意的作用。