Suppr超能文献

工具使用在数秒内改变多感官交互:来自跨通道一致性任务的证据。

Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task.

作者信息

Holmes Nicholas P, Calvert Gemma A, Spence Charles

机构信息

Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK.

出版信息

Exp Brain Res. 2007 Dec;183(4):465-76. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1060-7. Epub 2007 Jul 31.

Abstract

Active tool use in human and non-human primates has been claimed to alter the neural representations of multisensory peripersonal space. To date, most studies suggest that a short period of tool use leads to an expansion or elongation of these spatial representations, which lasts several minutes after the last tool use action. However, the possibility that multisensory interactions also change on a much shorter time scale following or preceding individual tool use movements has not yet been investigated. We measured crossmodal (visual-tactile) congruency effects as an index of multisensory integration during two tool use tasks. In the regular tool use task, the participants used one of two tools in a spatiotemporally predictable sequence after every fourth crossmodal congruency trial. In the random tool use task, the required timing and spatial location of the tool use task varied unpredictably. Multisensory integration effects increased as a function of the number of trials since tool use in the regular tool use group, but remained relatively constant in the random tool use group. The spatial distribution of these multisensory effects, however, was unaffected by tool use predictability, with significant spatial interactions found only near the hands and at the tips of the tools. These data suggest that endogenously preparing to use a tool enhances visual-tactile interactions near the tools. Such enhancements are likely due to the increased behavioural relevance of visual stimuli as each tool use action is prepared before execution.

摘要

人类和非人类灵长类动物主动使用工具被认为会改变多感觉近体空间的神经表征。迄今为止,大多数研究表明,短时间的工具使用会导致这些空间表征的扩展或延长,在最后一次工具使用动作后会持续几分钟。然而,在个体工具使用动作之前或之后,多感觉交互是否也会在更短的时间尺度上发生变化,这一可能性尚未得到研究。我们在两项工具使用任务中测量了跨模态(视觉 - 触觉)一致性效应,作为多感觉整合的指标。在常规工具使用任务中,参与者在每四次跨模态一致性试验后,按照时空可预测的顺序使用两种工具中的一种。在随机工具使用任务中,工具使用任务所需的时间和空间位置变化不可预测。在常规工具使用组中,多感觉整合效应随着自工具使用以来试验次数的增加而增加,但在随机工具使用组中保持相对恒定。然而,这些多感觉效应的空间分布不受工具使用可预测性的影响,仅在手部附近和工具尖端发现了显著的空间交互作用。这些数据表明,内源性地准备使用工具会增强工具附近的视觉 - 触觉交互。这种增强可能是由于在执行每个工具使用动作之前,视觉刺激的行为相关性增加。

相似文献

1
Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task.
Exp Brain Res. 2007 Dec;183(4):465-76. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1060-7. Epub 2007 Jul 31.
3
Tool-use: capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space?
Cortex. 2007 Apr;43(3):469-89. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70471-4.
4
The Black Book of Psychotropic Dosing and Monitoring.
Psychopharmacol Bull. 2024 Jul 8;54(3):8-59.
5
Neuromuscular electrical stimulation (NMES) for patellofemoral pain syndrome.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017 Dec 12;12(12):CD011289. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011289.pub2.
6
Reading aids for adults with low vision.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018 Apr 17;4(4):CD003303. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003303.pub4.
7
Thrombolysis for acute ischaemic stroke.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003(3):CD000213. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000213.
8
Interventions to reduce harm from continued tobacco use.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016 Oct 13;10(10):CD005231. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005231.pub3.
9
Electric fans for reducing adverse health impacts in heatwaves.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012 Jul 11;2012(7):CD009888. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009888.pub2.

引用本文的文献

2
Knowing your boundaries: no effect of tool-use on body representation following a gather-and-sort task.
Exp Brain Res. 2023 Sep;241(9):2275-2285. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06669-8. Epub 2023 Aug 8.
3
The different impact of attention, movement, and sensory information on body metric representation.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2024 May;77(5):1044-1051. doi: 10.1177/17470218231187385. Epub 2023 Jul 22.
4
Peripersonal space around the upper and the lower limbs.
Exp Brain Res. 2022 Aug;240(7-8):2039-2050. doi: 10.1007/s00221-022-06387-7. Epub 2022 Jun 21.
6
The toolish hand illusion: embodiment of a tool based on similarity with the hand.
Sci Rep. 2021 Jan 21;11(1):2024. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81706-6.
7
The Embodiment of Objects: Review, Analysis, and Future Directions.
Front Neurosci. 2019 Dec 13;13:1332. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01332. eCollection 2019.
8
Crossmodal congruency effect scores decrease with repeat test exposure.
PeerJ. 2019 May 22;7:e6976. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6976. eCollection 2019.
9
Different tool training induces specific effects on body metric representation.
Exp Brain Res. 2019 Feb;237(2):493-501. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5405-1. Epub 2018 Nov 20.
10
An Action Field Theory of Peripersonal Space.
Trends Cogn Sci. 2018 Dec;22(12):1076-1090. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.09.004. Epub 2018 Oct 15.

本文引用的文献

1
3
Tool-use: capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space?
Cortex. 2007 Apr;43(3):469-89. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70471-4.
5
Temporal dynamics of visuo-tactile extinction within and between hemispaces.
Neuropsychology. 2007 Mar;21(2):242-50. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.242.
7
Covert manual response preparation triggers attentional modulations of visual but not auditory processing.
Clin Neurophysiol. 2006 May;117(5):1063-74. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.005. Epub 2006 Mar 3.
8
The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space.
Cogn Process. 2004 Jun;5(2):94-105. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3.
9
Revisiting unilateral neglect.
Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(6):987-1006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.004. Epub 2005 Nov 21.
10
Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior.
Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(6):845-59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.009. Epub 2005 Nov 8.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验