Janssen Steve M J, Kristo Gert, Rouw Romke, Murre Jaap M J
School of Psychology, Flinders University, Australia.
School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Tilburg University, Netherlands.
Conscious Cogn. 2015 Jan;31:12-23. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.001. Epub 2014 Oct 24.
The basic-systems approach (Rubin, 2005, 2006) states that autobiographical memory is supported by other cognitive systems and argues that autobiographical memories are constructed from interactions between cognitive systems, such as language, vision and emotion. Although deficiencies in one or more of the basic systems influence the properties of autobiographical memories, little is known about how these cognitive abilities and autobiographical memory are related. To assert whether participants with stronger cognitive abilities also perform better on autobiographical memory tests, participants who completed verbal and visuospatial memory tests also recorded one personal event, which they recalled after a certain interval. Participants who performed well on the verbal memory tests also had better retention for the personal event, providing support for the basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory and preliminary support for the view that people have more memories from adolescence and early adulthood because the memory system works optimally in these lifetime periods.
基本系统方法(鲁宾,2005年、2006年)指出,自传体记忆受到其他认知系统的支持,并认为自传体记忆是由认知系统之间的相互作用构建而成的,比如语言、视觉和情感。虽然一个或多个基本系统的缺陷会影响自传体记忆的特性,但对于这些认知能力与自传体记忆之间的关系却知之甚少。为了确定认知能力较强的参与者在自传体记忆测试中是否也表现得更好,完成了言语和视觉空间记忆测试的参与者还记录了一件个人事件,并在一定间隔后进行回忆。在言语记忆测试中表现出色的参与者对个人事件的记忆保持效果也更好,这为自传体记忆的基本系统方法提供了支持,并初步支持了这样一种观点,即人们在青春期和成年早期有更多的记忆,因为记忆系统在这些人生阶段运作得最为理想。