Ryskin Rachel A, Qi Zhenghan, Duff Melissa C, Brown-Schmidt Sarah
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2017 May;43(5):781-794. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000341. Epub 2016 Oct 20.
Verbs often participate in more than 1 syntactic structure, but individual verbs can be biased in terms of whether they are used more often with 1 structure or the other. For instance, in a sentence such as "Bop the bunny with the flower," the phrase "with the flower" is more likely to indicate an instrument with which to "bop," rather than which "bunny" to bop. Conversely, in a sentence such as "Choose the cow with the flower," the phrase "with the flower" is more likely to indicate which "cow" to choose. An open question is where these biases come from and whether they continue to be shaped in adulthood in a way that has lasting consequences for real-time processing of language. In Experiment 1 we replicated previous findings that these language-wide biases guide online syntactic processing in a computer-based visual-world paradigm. In Experiment 2, we tested the malleability of these biases by exposing adults to initially unbiased verbs situated in unambiguous contexts that led to either instrument or modifier interpretations. During test, participants interpreted sentences containing either modifier- or instrument-trained verbs in ambiguous contexts. Eye-movement and action data show that participants' considerations of the candidate interpretations of the ambiguous with-phrases were guided by the newly learned verb biases. These results suggest that co-occurrence information about specific verbs and syntactic structures embedded in language experiences plays a role in forming, and continuously shaping, the verb biases that constitute a part of the broader representation of the language. (PsycINFO Database Record
动词常常参与不止一种句法结构,但就其更常与哪种结构搭配使用而言,单个动词可能存在偏向性。例如,在“用花轻拍兔子”这样的句子中,“用花”这个短语更有可能表示用来“轻拍”的工具,而不是要轻拍哪只“兔子”。相反,在“选择有花的奶牛”这样的句子中,“有花”这个短语更有可能表示要选择哪头“奶牛”。一个尚未解决的问题是,这些偏向性从何而来,以及它们在成年后是否仍会以一种对语言实时处理产生持久影响的方式持续形成。在实验1中,我们重复了之前的研究结果,即这些全语言范围的偏向性在基于计算机的视觉世界范式中指导在线句法处理。在实验2中,我们通过让成年人接触处于明确语境中的最初无偏向性的动词来测试这些偏向性的可塑性,这些语境会导致工具或修饰语的解释。在测试过程中,参与者在模糊语境中解释包含经过修饰语或工具训练的动词的句子。眼动和行为数据表明,参与者对模糊的“with”短语候选解释的考虑受到新习得的动词偏向性的引导。这些结果表明,语言经验中嵌入的关于特定动词和句法结构的共现信息在形成并持续塑造构成语言更广泛表征一部分的动词偏向性方面发挥了作用。(《心理学文摘数据库记录》 )