Suppr超能文献

想象本可能发生的事情:反事实思维的类型与生动程度及其与创伤后应激反应的关系。

Imagining What Could Have Happened: Types and Vividness of Counterfactual Thoughts and the Relationship With Post-traumatic Stress Reactions.

作者信息

Blix Ines, Kanten Alf Børre, Birkeland Marianne Skogbrott, Thoresen Siri

机构信息

Norwegian Centre for Violence and Traumatic Stress Studies, Oslo, Norway.

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.

出版信息

Front Psychol. 2018 Apr 20;9:515. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00515. eCollection 2018.

Abstract

A growing body of research suggests that counterfactual thinking after traumatic events is associated with post-traumatic stress reactions. In this study we explored frequency of upward and downward counterfactuals in trauma-exposed individuals, and how trauma-related counterfactuals were represented in terms of vividness. We examined the relationships between vividness and frequency of counterfactual thoughts and post-traumatic stress reactions in two groups who had experienced different types of traumatic exposure, namely survivors and bereaved from the fire on the ferry Scandinavian Star in 1990. Even after 26 years, both survivors and bereaved reported that they currently entertained thoughts about what could have happened during the fire on Scandinavian Star. Survivors reported more downward counterfactuals than the bereaved, whereas the bereaved reported more upward counterfactuals than the survivors did. Vividness of counterfactual thoughts, as well as reported frequency of upward and downward counterfactuals, were associated with post-traumatic stress reactions. Our results suggest that both upward and downward counterfactuals can be harmful, and that vivid counterfactuals about a traumatic event might play a similar role in post-traumatic stress as trauma memories. Therefore, traumatized individuals who entertain counterfactual thoughts may benefit from interventions that target these thoughts specifically.

摘要

越来越多的研究表明,创伤事件后的反事实思维与创伤后应激反应有关。在本研究中,我们探讨了经历创伤的个体向上和向下反事实思维的频率,以及与创伤相关的反事实思维在生动程度方面是如何呈现的。我们考察了两组经历不同类型创伤暴露的人群,即1990年斯堪的纳维亚之星号渡轮火灾的幸存者和遇难者家属,反事实思维的生动程度和频率与创伤后应激反应之间的关系。即使在26年之后,幸存者和遇难者家属都表示,他们目前仍会思考在斯堪的纳维亚之星号渡轮火灾期间可能发生的事情。幸存者报告的向下反事实思维比遇难者家属更多,而遇难者家属报告的向上反事实思维比幸存者更多。反事实思维的生动程度,以及报告的向上和向下反事实思维的频率,都与创伤后应激反应有关。我们的研究结果表明,向上和向下反事实思维都可能有害,而且关于创伤事件的生动反事实思维在创伤后应激中可能与创伤记忆起到类似的作用。因此,怀有反事实思维的受创伤个体可能会从专门针对这些思维的干预措施中受益。

相似文献

2
'My child could have died': counterfactual thoughts and psychological distress in parents of trauma survivors.
Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2024;15(1):2326736. doi: 10.1080/20008066.2024.2326736. Epub 2024 Mar 20.
3
Upward and Downward Counterfactual Thought After Loss: A Multiwave Controlled Longitudinal Study.
Behav Ther. 2021 May;52(3):577-593. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2020.07.007. Epub 2020 Aug 1.
5
Warning signals? An explorative study of distant recollections of the moments before a disaster.
Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2023;14(1):2180947. doi: 10.1080/20008066.2023.2180947.
6
Looking on the bright side: downward counterfactual thinking in response to negative life events.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005 Oct;31(10):1413-24. doi: 10.1177/0146167205276064.
7
Emotional intensity in episodic autobiographical memory and counterfactual thinking.
Conscious Cogn. 2017 Feb;48:283-291. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.12.013. Epub 2017 Jan 9.
8
Procrastination and counterfactual thinking: avoiding what might have been.
Br J Soc Psychol. 2004 Jun;43(Pt 2):269-86. doi: 10.1348/0144666041501660.
10
Improving workplace safety by thinking about what might have been: A first look at the role of counterfactual thinking.
J Safety Res. 2020 Feb;72:153-164. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2019.12.010. Epub 2020 Jan 3.

引用本文的文献

2
Maladaptive appraisals and posttraumatic stress reactions in young terror survivors across 8 years: a random intercepts cross-lagged analysis.
Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2025 Dec;16(1):2459462. doi: 10.1080/20008066.2025.2459462. Epub 2025 Feb 10.
4
'My child could have died': counterfactual thoughts and psychological distress in parents of trauma survivors.
Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2024;15(1):2326736. doi: 10.1080/20008066.2024.2326736. Epub 2024 Mar 20.
5
Individual differences in the frequency of voluntary & involuntary episodic memories, future thoughts, and counterfactual thoughts.
Psychol Res. 2023 Oct;87(7):2171-2182. doi: 10.1007/s00426-023-01802-2. Epub 2023 Feb 13.
8
If only… a systematic review and meta-analysis of social, temporal and counterfactual comparative thinking in PTSD.
Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2020 Apr 2;11(1):1737453. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2020.1737453. eCollection 2020.

本文引用的文献

2
The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5): Development and Initial Psychometric Evaluation.
J Trauma Stress. 2015 Dec;28(6):489-98. doi: 10.1002/jts.22059. Epub 2015 Nov 25.
3
A magnitude effect in judgments of subjective closeness.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015 Dec;41(12):1712-22. doi: 10.1177/0146167215609894. Epub 2015 Oct 13.
4
Unique relations between counterfactual thinking and DSM-5 PTSD symptom clusters.
Psychol Trauma. 2016 May;8(3):293-300. doi: 10.1037/tra0000089. Epub 2015 Oct 12.
5
Rethinking Rumination.
Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008 Sep;3(5):400-24. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.
6
Clinical applications of counterfactual thinking during memory reactivation.
Behav Brain Sci. 2015;38:e5. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000351.
8
Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control.
Psychol Res. 2014 Sep;78(5):651-60. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5.
9
Remembering what could have happened: neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking.
Neuropsychologia. 2013 Oct;51(12):2401-14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.015. Epub 2013 Jan 31.
10
Counterfactual thinking: an fMRI study on changing the past for a better future.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013 Jun;8(5):556-64. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss031. Epub 2012 Mar 7.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验