Meissner Franziska, Grigutsch Laura Anne, Koranyi Nicolas, Müller Florian, Rothermund Klaus
General Psychology II, Institute of Psychology, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Jena, Germany.
Department for the Psychology of Human Movement and Sport, Institute for Sports Science, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Jena, Germany.
Front Psychol. 2019 Nov 8;10:2483. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02483. eCollection 2019.
Two decades ago, the introduction of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) sparked enthusiastic reactions. With implicit measures like the IAT, researchers hoped to finally be able to bridge the gap between self-reported attitudes on one hand and behavior on the other. Twenty years of research and several meta-analyses later, however, we have to conclude that neither the IAT nor its derivatives have fulfilled these expectations. Their predictive value for behavioral criteria is weak and their incremental validity over and above self-report measures is negligible. In our review, we present an overview of explanations for these unsatisfactory findings and delineate promising ways forward. Over the years, several reasons for the IAT's weak predictive validity have been proposed. They point to four potentially problematic features: First, the IAT is by no means a pure measure of individual differences in associations but suffers from extraneous influences like recoding. Hence, the predictive validity of IAT-scores should not be confused with the predictive validity of associations. Second, with the IAT, we usually aim to measure evaluation ("liking") instead of motivation ("wanting"). Yet, behavior might be determined much more often by the latter than the former. Third, the IAT focuses on measuring associations instead of propositional beliefs and thus taps into a construct that might be too unspecific to account for behavior. Finally, studies on predictive validity are often characterized by a mismatch between predictor and criterion (e.g., while behavior is highly context-specific, the IAT usually takes into account neither the situation nor the domain). Recent research, however, also revealed advances addressing each of these problems, namely (1) procedural and analytical advances to control for recoding in the IAT, (2) measurement procedures to assess implicit wanting, (3) measurement procedures to assess implicit beliefs, and (4) approaches to increase the fit between implicit measures and behavioral criteria (e.g., by incorporating contextual information). Implicit measures like the IAT hold an enormous potential. In order to allow them to fulfill this potential, however, we have to refine our understanding of these measures, and we should incorporate recent conceptual and methodological advancements. This review provides specific recommendations on how to do so.
二十年前,内隐联想测验(IAT)的引入引发了热烈反响。借助像IAT这样的内隐测量方法,研究人员希望最终能够弥合一方面的自我报告态度与另一方面的行为之间的差距。然而,经过二十年的研究以及多项元分析之后,我们不得不得出结论,无论是IAT还是其衍生方法都未能实现这些期望。它们对行为标准的预测价值微弱,相对于自我报告测量方法而言,其增量效度可忽略不计。在我们的综述中,我们概述了对这些不尽人意的研究结果的解释,并勾勒出了有前景的前进方向。多年来,人们提出了IAT预测效度薄弱的几个原因。这些原因指向四个潜在的问题特征:第一,IAT绝不是关联中个体差异的纯粹测量方法,而是受到像重新编码这样的外部影响。因此,IAT分数的预测效度不应与关联的预测效度相混淆。第二,使用IAT时,我们通常旨在测量评价(“喜欢”)而非动机(“想要”)。然而,行为可能更多地由后者而非前者决定。第三,IAT专注于测量关联而非命题信念,因此触及到一个可能过于不具体而无法解释行为的结构。最后,关于预测效度的研究往往以预测指标与标准之间的不匹配为特征(例如,虽然行为具有高度情境特异性,但IAT通常既不考虑情境也不考虑领域)。然而,最近的研究也揭示了解决这些问题的进展,即(1)控制IAT中重新编码的程序和分析进展,(2)评估内隐想要的测量程序,(3)评估内隐信念的测量程序,以及(4)提高内隐测量与行为标准之间契合度的方法(例如,通过纳入情境信息)。像IAT这样的内隐测量方法具有巨大潜力。然而,为了使其发挥这一潜力,我们必须完善对这些测量方法的理解,并且应该纳入最近的概念和方法进展。本综述提供了关于如何做到这一点的具体建议。