Suppr超能文献

幼儿在动词学习过程中能否忽略不相关的事件或子事件?

Can Young Children Ignore Irrelevant Events, or Subevents, During Verb Learning?

作者信息

Howard Tyler J, Porter Blaire M, Childers Jane B

机构信息

Trinity University.

出版信息

J Cogn Dev. 2019;20(3):411-432. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1607861. Epub 2019 May 13.

Abstract

Children learning a verb may benefit from hearing it across situations (e.g., Behrend, 1995; Childers, 2011; Fisher et al, 1994; Pinker, 1989). At the same time, in everyday contexts, situations in which a verb is heard will be interrupted by distracting events. Using Structural Alignment theory as a framework (e.g., Gentner & Namy, 2006), Study 1 asks whether children can learn a verb when irrelevant, interleaved events are present. Two½- and 3½-year-old children saw dynamic events and were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions (differing in orders of events), or one of two control conditions. They extended the verbs in the experimental conditions, and not the control conditions. Three ½-year-olds were more successful than 2½-year-olds, though the younger children could extend verbs. A more difficult task is segmenting dynamic action into subevents that could be relevant for a verb (e.g., finding "chopping" in a cooking scene). In Study 2, 2½-, 3½- and 4½-year-old children were assigned to experimental conditions in which relevant events flowed into irrelevant events (or vice versa), or to a control. Two½-year-olds failed to extend the verbs at test, differing from the older children; children in experimental conditions extended the verbs while children in the control condition did not. Altogether, these results show children can ignore irrelevant events (and subevents), and extend new verbs by 3½ years. Results are important to understand learning in everyday contexts in which verbs are heard in varied situations over time.

摘要

学习动词的儿童可能会从在不同情境中听到该动词中受益(例如,贝伦德,1995年;奇尔德斯,2011年;费舍尔等人,1994年;平克,1989年)。与此同时,在日常情境中,听到动词的情境会被干扰事件打断。以结构对齐理论为框架(例如,根特纳和纳米,2006年),研究1探讨了当存在无关的、交错的事件时,儿童是否能够学习动词。2.5岁和3.5岁的儿童观看了动态事件,并被随机分配到三个实验条件之一(事件顺序不同),或两个控制条件之一。他们在实验条件下扩展了动词,而在控制条件下没有。3.5岁的儿童比2.5岁的儿童更成功,尽管年幼的儿童也能扩展动词。一项更具挑战性的任务是将动态动作分割成与动词可能相关的子事件(例如,在烹饪场景中找出“切”的动作)。在研究2中,2.5岁、3.5岁和4.5岁的儿童被分配到相关事件融入无关事件(或反之亦然)的实验条件,或一个控制条件。2.5岁的儿童在测试时未能扩展动词,这与年龄较大的儿童不同;实验条件下的儿童扩展了动词,而控制条件下的儿童没有。总之,这些结果表明儿童能够忽略无关事件(和子事件),并在3.5岁时扩展新动词。这些结果对于理解在日常情境中随着时间推移在各种情境中听到动词的学习情况很重要。

相似文献

1
Can Young Children Ignore Irrelevant Events, or Subevents, During Verb Learning?
J Cogn Dev. 2019;20(3):411-432. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1607861. Epub 2019 May 13.
2
Early Verb Learning: How Do Children Learn How to Compare Events?
J Cogn Dev. 2016;17(1):41-66. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2015.1042580. Epub 2015 Jul 6.
3
Attention to Multiple Events Helps 2 1/2-Year-Olds Extend New Verbs.
First Lang. 2011 Feb 1;31(1). doi: 10.1177/0142723710361825.
4
Can children learn verbs from events separated in time? Examining how variability and memory contribute to verb learning.
J Exp Child Psychol. 2023 Mar;227:105583. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105583. Epub 2022 Nov 18.
5
The development of using function word "zài" to learn novel verbs in young Mandarin speakers.
Psych J. 2024 Feb;13(1):31-43. doi: 10.1002/pchj.710. Epub 2023 Dec 17.
6
Morphological cues to verb meaning: verb inflections and the initial mapping of verb meanings.
J Child Lang. 1995 Feb;22(1):89-106. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900009648.
7
Children Use Different Cues to Guide Noun and Verb Extensions.
Lang Learn Dev. 2012;8(3):233-254. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2011.585285.
8
Does Variability Across Events Affect Verb Learning in English, Mandarin, and Korean?
Cogn Sci. 2017 Apr;41 Suppl 4(Suppl 4):808-830. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12398. Epub 2016 Jul 25.
10
Acquisition of Verb Meaning From Syntactic Distribution in Preschoolers With Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2018 Aug 14;49(3S):668-680. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-STLT1-17-0126.

引用本文的文献

本文引用的文献

1
Analogy and Abstraction.
Top Cogn Sci. 2017 Jul;9(3):672-693. doi: 10.1111/tops.12278. Epub 2017 Jun 16.
2
Attention to Multiple Events Helps 2 1/2-Year-Olds Extend New Verbs.
First Lang. 2011 Feb 1;31(1). doi: 10.1177/0142723710361825.
3
Goals influence memory and imitation for dynamic human action in 36-month-old children.
Scand J Psychol. 2013 Feb;54(1):41-50. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12004. Epub 2012 Nov 2.
4
2.5-year-olds use cross-situational consistency to learn verbs under referential uncertainty.
Cognition. 2012 Feb;122(2):163-80. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.010. Epub 2011 Nov 20.
5
How words can and cannot be learned by observation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 May 31;108(22):9014-9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105040108. Epub 2011 May 16.
6
Meaning from syntax: evidence from 2-year-olds.
Cognition. 2010 Mar;114(3):442-6. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.015. Epub 2009 Nov 28.
7
Event boundaries in perception affect memory encoding and updating.
J Exp Psychol Gen. 2009 May;138(2):236-57. doi: 10.1037/a0015631.
8
Infants' ability to parse continuous actions.
Dev Psychol. 2009 Mar;45(2):575-85. doi: 10.1037/a0014145.
9
Korean- and English-speaking children use cross-situational information to learn novel predicate terms.
J Child Lang. 2009 Jan;36(1):201-24. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008891. Epub 2008 Aug 27.
10
Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children.
Child Dev. 2008 Jul-Aug;79(4):979-1000. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01171.x.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验