Suppr超能文献

协作联合行动中言语交流中沟通不畅的预测

Predictions of Miscommunication in Verbal Communication During Collaborative Joint Action.

作者信息

Paxton Alexandra, Roche Jennifer M, Ibarra Alyssa, Tanenhaus Michael K

机构信息

Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs.

Center for the Ecological Study of Perception and Action, University of Connecticut, Storrs.

出版信息

J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2021 Feb 17;64(2):613-627. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00137. Epub 2021 Jan 27.

Abstract

Purpose The purpose of the current study was to examine the lexical and pragmatic factors that may contribute to turn-by-turn failures in communication (i.e., miscommunication) that arise regularly in interactive communication. Method Using a corpus from a collaborative dyadic building task, we investigated what differentiated successful from unsuccessful communication and potential factors associated with the choice to provide greater lexical information to a conversation partner. Results We found that more successful dyads' language tended to be associated with greater lexical density, lower ambiguity, and fewer questions. We also found participants were more lexically dense when accepting and integrating a partner's information (i.e., grounding) but were less lexically dense when responding to a question. Finally, an exploratory analysis suggested that dyads tended to spend more lexical effort when responding to an inquiry and used assent language accurately-that is, only when communication was successful. Conclusion Together, the results suggest that miscommunication both emerges and benefits from ambiguous and lexically dense utterances.

摘要

目的 本研究的目的是考察那些可能导致在互动交流中经常出现的逐轮沟通失败(即沟通不畅)的词汇和语用因素。方法 我们使用了一个来自协作性二元建筑任务的语料库,调查了成功沟通与不成功沟通的差异,以及与选择向对话伙伴提供更多词汇信息相关的潜在因素。结果 我们发现,更成功的二元组的语言往往与更高的词汇密度、更低的歧义性和更少的问题相关。我们还发现,参与者在接受和整合伙伴的信息(即建立基础)时词汇密度更高,但在回答问题时词汇密度较低。最后,一项探索性分析表明,二元组在回答询问时往往会花费更多的词汇精力,并且准确地使用赞同性语言——也就是说,只有在沟通成功时才会这样。结论 综合来看,结果表明沟通不畅既源于模糊且词汇密集的话语,也受益于这些话语。

相似文献

1
Predictions of Miscommunication in Verbal Communication During Collaborative Joint Action.
J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2021 Feb 17;64(2):613-627. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00137. Epub 2021 Jan 27.
2
The Effects of Emotion Suppression During Language Planning and Production.
J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2018 Aug 8;61(8):2076-2083. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0232.
3
Evidence for a dyadic motor plan in joint action.
Sci Rep. 2018 Mar 22;8(1):5027. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23275-9.
5
Cognitive Processing of Miscommunication in Interactive Listening: An Evaluation of Listener Indecision and Cognitive Effort.
J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2021 Jan 14;64(1):159-175. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00128. Epub 2021 Jan 5.
6
Speakers' overestimation of their effectiveness.
Psychol Sci. 2002 May;13(3):207-12. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00439.
7
Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers.
Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2006 May-Jun;41(3):293-311. doi: 10.1080/13682820500342976.
8
Children's ambiguous utterances: a re-examination of processing limitations on production.
J Child Lang. 1995 Feb;22(1):151-69. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900009673.
9
Lexically guided perceptual learning is robust to task-based changes in listening strategy.
J Acoust Soc Am. 2018 Aug;144(2):1089. doi: 10.1121/1.5047672.
10
Communication in conversation in stroke patients.
J Neurol. 2010 Jul;257(7):1099-107. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5469-8. Epub 2010 Feb 9.

引用本文的文献

1
Developmental kindergarten classroom intervention for spatial relational terms.
Codas. 2022 May 9;34(6):e20210176. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20212021176. eCollection 2022.

本文引用的文献

1
The Role of Communication During Trauma Activations: Investigating the Need for Team and Leader Communication Training.
J Surg Educ. 2017 Jan-Feb;74(1):173-179. doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2016.06.001. Epub 2016 Jul 12.
2
The Flexibility of Conceptual Pacts: Referring Expressions Dynamically Shift to Accommodate New Conceptualizations.
Front Psychol. 2016 Apr 25;7:561. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00561. eCollection 2016.
3
Talker-Specific Generalization of Pragmatic Inferences based on Under- and Over-Informative Prenominal Adjective Use.
Front Psychol. 2016 Jan 20;6:2035. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02035. eCollection 2015.
4
Unpacking the Complexity of Patient Handoffs Through the Lens of Cognitive Load Theory.
Teach Learn Med. 2016;28(1):88-96. doi: 10.1080/10401334.2015.1107491.
5
Natural discourse reference generation reduces cognitive load in spoken systems.
Nat Lang Eng. 2011 Jul 1;17(3):311-329. doi: 10.1017/S1351324910000227.
6
Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal.
J Mem Lang. 2013 Apr;68(3). doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.
8
Coming to terms: quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination.
Psychol Sci. 2012 Aug 1;23(8):931-9. doi: 10.1177/0956797612436816. Epub 2012 Jul 17.
9
The communicative function of ambiguity in language.
Cognition. 2012 Mar;122(3):280-91. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.004. Epub 2011 Dec 20.
10
The half-life of cognitive-affective states during complex learning.
Cogn Emot. 2011 Nov;25(7):1299-308. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.613668. Epub 2011 Sep 26.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验