Krauzlis R J, Miles F A
Laboratory of Sensorimotor Research, National Eye Institute, Bethesda, Maryland 20892-4435, USA.
J Neurophysiol. 1998 Oct;80(4):2046-62. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.2046.
We studied the eye movements evoked by applying small amounts of current (2-50 microA) within the oculomotor vermis of two monkeys. We first compared the eye movements evoked by microstimulation applied either during maintained pursuit or during fixation. Smooth, pursuitlike changes in eye velocity caused by the microstimulation were directed toward the ipsilateral side and occurred at short latencies (10-20 ms). The amplitudes of these pursuitlike changes were larger during visually guided pursuit toward the contralateral side than during either fixation or visually guided pursuit toward the ipsilateral side. At these same sites, microstimulation also often produced abrupt, saccadelike changes in eye velocity. In contrast to the smooth changes in eye velocity, these saccadelike effects were more prevalent during fixation and during pursuit toward the ipsilateral side. The amplitude and type of evoked eye movements could also be manipulated at single sites by changing the frequency of microstimulation. Increasing the frequency of microstimulation produced increases in the amplitude of pursuitlike changes, but only up to a certain point. Beyond this point, the value of which depended on the site and whether the monkey was fixating or pursuing, further increases in stimulation frequency produced saccadelike changes of increasing amplitude. To quantify these effects, we introduced a novel method for classifying eye movements as pursuitlike or saccadelike. The results of this analysis showed that the eye movements evoked by microstimulation exhibit a distinct transition point between pursuit and saccadelike effects and that the amplitude of eye movement that corresponds to this transition point depends on the eye movement behavior of the monkey. These results are consistent with accumulating evidence that the oculomotor vermis and its associated deep cerebellar nucleus, the caudal fastigial, are involved in the control of both pursuit and saccadic eye movements. We suggest that the oculomotor vermis might accomplish this role by altering the amplitude of a motor error signal that is common to both saccades and pursuit.
我们研究了在两只猴子的动眼蚓部施加少量电流(2 - 50微安)所诱发的眼球运动。我们首先比较了在持续追踪或注视过程中微刺激诱发的眼球运动。微刺激引起的眼球速度的平滑、类似追踪的变化朝向同侧,且潜伏期较短(10 - 20毫秒)。与注视或向同侧的视觉引导追踪相比,在向对侧的视觉引导追踪过程中,这些类似追踪变化的幅度更大。在相同部位,微刺激还常常引起眼球速度的突然、类似扫视的变化。与眼球速度的平滑变化不同,这些类似扫视的效应在注视和向同侧追踪过程中更为普遍。通过改变微刺激的频率,也可以在单个部位操纵诱发眼球运动的幅度和类型。增加微刺激的频率会使类似追踪变化的幅度增加,但仅到一定程度。超过这一点(该点的值取决于部位以及猴子是在注视还是在追踪),刺激频率的进一步增加会产生幅度不断增加的类似扫视的变化。为了量化这些效应,我们引入了一种将眼球运动分类为类似追踪或类似扫视的新方法。该分析结果表明,微刺激诱发的眼球运动在类似追踪和类似扫视效应之间呈现出明显的转变点,并且与该转变点对应的眼球运动幅度取决于猴子的眼球运动行为。这些结果与越来越多的证据一致,即动眼蚓部及其相关的小脑深部核团——尾侧顶核,参与了追踪和扫视眼球运动的控制。我们认为,动眼蚓部可能通过改变扫视和追踪共同的运动误差信号的幅度来发挥这一作用。