Suppr超能文献

野生黑猩猩的关节协同运动中的发声募集。

Vocal recruitment for joint travel in wild chimpanzees.

机构信息

Centre Norbert Elias, EHESS, Marseille, France ; School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom ; Anthropological Institute, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland ; Budongo Conservation Field Station, Masindi, Uganda.

出版信息

PLoS One. 2013 Sep 25;8(9):e76073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076073. eCollection 2013.

Abstract

Joint travel is a common social activity of many group-living animals, which requires some degree of coordination, sometimes through communication signals. Here, we studied the use of an acoustically distinct vocalisation in chimpanzees, the 'travel hoo', a signal given specifically in the travel context. We were interested in how this call type was produced to coordinate travel, whether it was aimed at specific individuals and how recipients responded. We found that 'travel hoos' were regularly given prior to impending departures and that silent travel initiations were less successful in recruiting than vocal initiations. Other behaviours associated with departure were unrelated to recruitment, suggesting that 'travel hoos' facilitated joint travel. Crucially, 'travel hoos' were more often produced in the presence of allies than other individuals, with high rates of recruitment success. We discuss these findings as evidence for how motivation to perform a specific social activity can lead to the production of a vocal signal that qualifies as 'intentional' according to most definitions, suggesting that a key psychological component of human language may have already been present in the common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans.

摘要

关节运动是许多群居动物的一种常见社交活动,需要一定程度的协调,有时通过沟通信号。在这里,我们研究了黑猩猩使用一种独特的声音——“旅行呼”,这是一种在旅行环境中发出的特定信号。我们感兴趣的是这种叫声是如何产生以协调旅行的,它是否针对特定的个体,以及接收者的反应如何。我们发现,“旅行呼”通常在即将出发前发出,而无声的旅行启动比有声的启动招募效果差。与出发相关的其他行为与招募无关,这表明“旅行呼”促进了联合旅行。至关重要的是,“旅行呼”在盟友在场的情况下比其他个体更频繁地发出,招募成功率很高。我们认为这些发现证明了执行特定社交活动的动机如何导致产生一种被大多数定义为“有意”的声音信号,这表明人类语言的一个关键心理组成部分可能已经存在于黑猩猩和人类的共同祖先中。

相似文献

1
Vocal recruitment for joint travel in wild chimpanzees.
PLoS One. 2013 Sep 25;8(9):e76073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076073. eCollection 2013.
2
An intentional cohesion call in male chimpanzees of Budongo Forest.
Anim Cogn. 2022 Aug;25(4):853-866. doi: 10.1007/s10071-022-01597-6. Epub 2022 Jan 19.
3
An intentional vocalization draws others' attention: a playback experiment with wild chimpanzees.
Anim Cogn. 2015 May;18(3):581-91. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0827-z. Epub 2014 Dec 24.
4
Chimpanzee quiet hoo variants differ according to context.
R Soc Open Sci. 2018 May 23;5(5):172066. doi: 10.1098/rsos.172066. eCollection 2018 May.
5
Vocal signature in wild infant chimpanzees.
Am J Primatol. 2013 Apr;75(4):324-32. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22108. Epub 2012 Dec 10.
6
Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality.
PLoS One. 2013 Oct 16;8(10):e76674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674. eCollection 2013.
7
Multimodal communication in chimpanzees.
Am J Primatol. 2015 Nov;77(11):1143-8. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22449. Epub 2015 Jul 23.

引用本文的文献

1
Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) mentally represent collaboration?: Action-learning and communication in a partnered task.
PLoS One. 2025 Jun 6;20(6):e0325418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0325418. eCollection 2025.
2
Versatile use of chimpanzee call combinations promotes meaning expansion.
Sci Adv. 2025 May 9;11(19):eadq2879. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.adq2879.
3
Vocal Learning Versus Speech Evolution: Untangling a False Equivalence.
Ecol Evol. 2025 Apr 3;15(4):e71241. doi: 10.1002/ece3.71241. eCollection 2025 Apr.
4
A non-human primate combinatorial system for long-distance communication.
iScience. 2024 Oct 15;27(11):111172. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.111172. eCollection 2024 Nov 15.
5
Vocal consensus building for collective departures in wild western gorillas.
Proc Biol Sci. 2024 Oct;291(2033):20240597. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2024.0597. Epub 2024 Oct 23.
6
Exploring the role of vocalizations in regulating group dynamics.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2024 Jul 8;379(1905):20230183. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2023.0183. Epub 2024 May 20.
9
Communicative roots of complex sociality and cognition: preface to the theme issue.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2022 Sep 26;377(1860):20220115. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0115. Epub 2022 Aug 8.
10
Interaction and ostension: the myth of 4th-order intentionality.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2022 Sep 12;377(1859):20210105. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0105. Epub 2022 Jul 25.

本文引用的文献

1
Referential gestures in fish collaborative hunting.
Nat Commun. 2013;4:1765. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2781.
3
Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees.
Proc Biol Sci. 2013 Jan 23;280(1755):20122765. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2765. Print 2013 Mar 22.
4
Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition.
PLoS One. 2008;3(6):e2431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002431. Epub 2008 Jun 18.
5
Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger.
Curr Biol. 2012 Jan 24;22(2):142-6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053. Epub 2011 Dec 29.
6
The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild.
Nat Commun. 2011 Nov 29;2:560. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1567.
7
The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee.
Anim Cogn. 2011 Sep;14(5):745-67. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2. Epub 2011 May 1.
8
Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates.
Am J Primatol. 2011 Jun;73(6):530-44. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20913. Epub 2010 Dec 22.
9
Female bonobos use copulation calls as social signals.
Biol Lett. 2011 Aug 23;7(4):513-6. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.1227. Epub 2011 Feb 16.
10
Bonobos voluntarily share their own food with others.
Curr Biol. 2010 Mar 9;20(5):R230-1. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.038.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验