Oller D Kimbrough, Griebel Ulrike, Iyer Suneeti Nathani, Jhang Yuna, Warlaumont Anne S, Dale Rick, Call Josep
School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN, United States.
Institute for Intelligent Systems, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN, United States.
Front Psychol. 2019 Apr 2;10:729. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00729. eCollection 2019.
From the first months of life, human infants produce "protophones," speech-like, non-cry sounds, presumed absent, or only minimally present in other apes. But there have been no direct quantitative comparisons to support this presumption. In addition, by 2 months, human infants show sustained face-to-face interaction using protophones, a pattern thought also absent or very limited in other apes, but again, without quantitative comparison. Such comparison should provide evidence relevant to determining foundations of language, since substantially flexible vocalization, the inclination to explore vocalization, and the ability to interact socially by means of vocalization are foundations for language. Here we quantitatively compare data on vocalization rates in three captive bonobo () mother-infant pairs with various sources of data from our laboratories on human infant vocalization. Both humans and bonobos produced distress sounds (cries/screams) and laughter. The bonobo infants also produced sounds that were neither screams nor laughs and that showed acoustic similarities to the human protophones. These protophone-like sounds confirm that bonobo infants share with humans the capacity to produce vocalizations that appear foundational for language. Still, there were dramatic differences between the species in both quantity and function of the protophone and protophone-like sounds. The bonobo protophone-like sounds were far less frequent than the human protophones, and the human protophones were far less likely to be interpreted as complaints and more likely as vocal play. Moreover, we found extensive vocal interaction between human infants and mothers, but vocal interaction in the bonobo mother-infant pairs-while bonobo mothers were physically responsive to their infants, we observed no case of a bonobo mother vocalization directed to her infant. Our cross-species comparison focuses on low- and moderate-arousal circumstances because we reason the roots of language entail vocalization not triggered by excitement, for example, during fighting or intense play. Language appears to be founded in flexible vocalization, used to regulate comfortable social interaction, to share variable affective states at various levels of arousal, and to explore vocalization itself.
从出生后的最初几个月起,人类婴儿就会发出“原始语音”,即类似言语的非哭声,据推测其他猿类没有这种声音,或者只有极少的这种声音。但一直没有直接的定量比较来支持这一推测。此外,到2个月大时,人类婴儿会使用原始语音进行持续的面对面互动,这种模式在其他猿类中也被认为不存在或非常有限,但同样没有进行定量比较。这样的比较应该能提供与确定语言基础相关的证据,因为高度灵活的发声、探索发声的倾向以及通过发声进行社交互动的能力是语言的基础。在这里,我们对三只圈养倭黑猩猩母婴对的发声频率数据与我们实验室关于人类婴儿发声的各种数据来源进行了定量比较。人类和倭黑猩猩都会发出痛苦的声音(哭声/尖叫声)和笑声。倭黑猩猩婴儿还会发出既不是尖叫声也不是笑声的声音,这些声音在声学上与人类的原始语音相似。这些类似原始语音的声音证实了倭黑猩猩婴儿与人类一样具备发出似乎是语言基础的发声的能力。然而,在原始语音和类似原始语音的声音的数量和功能方面,这两个物种之间存在巨大差异。倭黑猩猩类似原始语音的声音比人类的原始语音频率低得多,而且人类的原始语音被解读为抱怨的可能性要小得多,更有可能被视为发声游戏。此外,我们发现人类婴儿和母亲之间有广泛的发声互动,但在倭黑猩猩母婴对中却没有发声互动——虽然倭黑猩猩母亲对婴儿有身体上的反应,但我们没有观察到倭黑猩猩母亲对婴儿发声的情况。我们的跨物种比较集中在低唤醒和中等唤醒的情况下,因为我们认为语言的根源在于不是由兴奋引发的发声,例如在打架或激烈玩耍时。语言似乎建立在灵活的发声基础上,用于调节舒适的社交互动、在不同唤醒水平下分享不同的情感状态以及探索发声本身。
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021-10-25
Sci Rep. 2019-10-14
Front Psychol. 2017-3-24
Front Commun (Lausanne). 2017
Top Cogn Sci. 2016-4
Infant Behav Dev. 2021-11
PeerJ. 2015-8-4
Sci Rep. 2025-3-12
Front Integr Neurosci. 2024-12-11
J Autism Dev Disord. 2025-3
Biling (Camb Engl). 2023-11
Animals (Basel). 2018-7-23
Attach Hum Dev. 2025-6
Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2017-5-17
Front Psychol. 2017-3-24
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016-12-5