迈向政治发展科学。

Toward a Developmental Science of Politics.

机构信息

University of Kansas.

University of Texas at Austin.

出版信息

Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2019 Sep;84(3):7-185. doi: 10.1111/mono.12410.

Abstract

In this monograph, we argue for the establishment of a developmental science of politics that describes, explains, and predicts the formation and change of individuals' political knowledge, attitudes, and behavior beginning in childhood and continuing across the life course. Reflecting our goal of contributing both theoretical conceptualizations and empirical data, we have organized the monograph into two broad sections. In the first section, we outline theoretical contributions that the study of politics may make to developmental science and provide practical reasons that empirical research in the domain of politics is important (e.g., for identifying ways to improve civics education and for encouraging higher voting rates among young adults). We also review major historical approaches to the study of political development and provide an integrative theoretical framework to ground future work. Drawing on Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems model as an organizing scheme and emphasizing social justice issues, we describe how factors rooted in cultural contexts, families, and children themselves are likely to shape political development. In the second section of the monograph, we argue for the importance and utility of studying major political events, such as presidential elections, and introduce the major themes, rationales, and hypotheses for a study of U.S. children's views of the 2016 U.S. presidential election. In addition, we apply a social-justice lens to political thought and participation, addressing the role of gender/sex and race/ethnicity in children's political development broadly, and in their knowledge and views of the 2016 U.S. presidential election specifically. In interviews conducted within the month before and after the election, we examined two overarching categories of children's political attitudes: (a) knowledge, preferences, and expectations about the 2016 election, and (b) knowledge and attitudes concerning gender/sex and politics, particularly relevant for the 2016 election given Hillary Clinton's role as the first female major-party candidate for the presidency. Participants were 187 children (101 girls) between 5 and 11 years of age (M = 8.42 years, SD = 1.45 years). They were recruited from schools and youth organizations in five counties in four U.S. states (Kansas, Kentucky, Texas, and Washington) with varying voting patterns (e.g., Trump voters ranged from 27% to 71% of county voters). The sample was not a nationally representative one, but was racially diverse (35 African American, 50 Latinx, 81 White, and 21 multiracial, Asian American, Middle Eastern, or Native American children). In addition to several child characteristics (e.g., age, social dominance orientation [SDO]), we assessed several family and community characteristics (e.g., child-reported parental interest in the election and government-reported county-level voting patterns, respectively) hypothesized to predict outcome variables. Although our findings are shaped by the nature of our sample (e.g., our participants were less likely to support Trump than children in larger, nationwide samples were), they offer preliminary insights into children's political development. Overall, children in our sample were interested in and knowledgeable about the presidential election (e.g., a large majority identified the candidates correctly and reported some knowledge about their personal qualities or policy positions). They reported more information about Donald Trump's than Hillary Clinton's policies, largely accounted for by the substantial percentage of children (41%) who referred to Trump's immigration policies (e.g., building a wall between the United States and Mexico). Overall, children reported as many negative as positive personal qualities of the candidates, with negative qualities being reported more often for Trump than for Clinton (56% and 18% of children, respectively). Most children (88%) supported Clinton over Trump, a preference that did not vary by participants' gender/sex or race/ethnicity. In their responses to an open-ended inquiry about their reactions to Trump's win, 63% of children reported negative and 18% reported positive emotions. Latinx children reacted more negatively to the election outcome than did White children. Girls' and boys' emotional responses to the election outcome did not differ. Children's personal interest in serving as U.S. president did not vary across gender/sex or racial/ethnic groups (overall, 42% were interested). Clinton's loss of the election did not appear to depress (or pique) girls' interest in becoming U.S. president. With respect to the role of gender/sex in politics, many children (35%) were ignorant about women's absence from the U.S. presidency. Only a single child was able to name a historical individual who worked for women's civil rights or suffrage. Child characteristics predicted some outcome variables. For example, as expected, older children showed greater knowledge about the candidates than did younger children. Family and community characteristics also predicted some outcome variables. For example, as expected, participants were more likely to support Trump if they perceived that their parents supported him and if Trump received a greater percentage of votes in the children's county of residence. Our data suggest that civic education should be expanded and reformed. In addition to addressing societal problems requiring political solutions, civics lessons should include the histories of social groups' political participation, including information about gender discrimination and the women's suffrage movement in U.S. political history. Providing children with environments that are rich in information related to the purpose and value of politics, and with opportunities and encouragement for political thought and action, is potentially beneficial for youth and their nations.

摘要

在这本专论中,我们主张建立一个政治发展科学,该科学描述、解释和预测个体的政治知识、态度和行为的形成和变化,从儿童时期开始,并贯穿整个生命周期。反映我们在理论概念化和实证数据两方面做出贡献的目标,我们将专论分为两个广泛的部分。在第一部分中,我们概述了政治学研究可能为发展科学做出的理论贡献,并提供了实证研究在政治学领域重要性的实际原因(例如,为了确定如何改进公民教育和鼓励年轻人更高的投票率)。我们还回顾了政治发展研究的主要历史方法,并提供了一个综合的理论框架来为未来的工作打下基础。我们借鉴了 Bronfenbrenner 的生态系统模型作为组织方案,并强调社会正义问题,描述了根植于文化背景、家庭和儿童自身的因素如何塑造政治发展。在专论的第二部分中,我们主张研究重大政治事件(如总统选举)的重要性和实用性,并介绍了一项关于美国儿童对 2016 年美国总统选举看法的研究的主要主题、理由和假设。此外,我们从社会正义的角度探讨政治思想和参与,广泛涉及性别/性别和种族/族裔在儿童政治发展中的作用,以及他们对 2016 年美国总统选举的具体看法。在选举前后一个月内进行的访谈中,我们研究了儿童政治态度的两个总体类别:(a)对 2016 年选举的知识、偏好和预期,以及(b)对性别/性别和政治的知识和态度,特别是鉴于希拉里·克林顿作为第一位主要政党女性候选人参选,这与 2016 年的选举有很大关系。参与者是来自美国五个州(堪萨斯州、肯塔基州、德克萨斯州和华盛顿州)五个县的 187 名儿童(101 名女孩),年龄在 5 至 11 岁之间(M=8.42 岁,SD=1.45 岁)。他们是从学校和青年组织招募的,这些学校和组织所在的县投票模式各不相同(例如,特朗普的选民占县选民的 27%至 71%)。该样本不是全国代表性的样本,但具有种族多样性(35 名非裔美国人、50 名拉丁裔、81 名白人,以及 21 名多种族、亚裔美国人、中东裔或美洲原住民儿童)。除了一些儿童特征(如年龄、社会支配倾向[SDO])外,我们还评估了一些家庭和社区特征(如儿童报告的父母对选举的兴趣和政府报告的县一级投票模式),这些特征被假设可以预测结果变量。尽管我们的发现受到样本性质的影响(例如,我们的参与者支持特朗普的可能性低于全国更大的样本),但它们提供了儿童政治发展的初步见解。总体而言,我们样本中的儿童对总统选举很感兴趣且有一定了解(例如,绝大多数儿童正确识别了候选人,并对他们的个人品质或政策立场有一定的了解)。他们报告了更多关于唐纳德·特朗普的政策信息,而不是关于希拉里·克林顿的政策信息,这主要是由于相当一部分儿童(41%)提到了特朗普的移民政策(例如,在美国和墨西哥之间建造一堵墙)。总体而言,儿童对候选人的个人品质的评价既有正面的,也有负面的,对特朗普的负面评价多于对克林顿的负面评价(分别为 56%和 18%的儿童)。大多数儿童(88%)支持克林顿而不是特朗普,这种偏好不因参与者的性别/性别或种族/族裔而有所不同。在对特朗普获胜的反应的开放式询问中,63%的儿童报告了负面情绪,18%报告了正面情绪。拉丁裔儿童对选举结果的反应比白人儿童更为负面。女孩和男孩对选举结果的情绪反应没有差异。儿童对担任美国总统的个人兴趣在性别/性别或种族/族裔群体之间没有差异(总体而言,42%的儿童有兴趣)。克林顿输掉选举似乎并没有降低(或激发)女孩成为美国总统的兴趣。关于性别/性别在政治中的作用,许多儿童(35%)对女性缺席美国总统职位一无所知。只有一个孩子能够说出一个为争取妇女公民权利或选举权而努力的历史人物。儿童特征预测了一些结果变量。例如,正如预期的那样,年龄较大的儿童比年龄较小的儿童对候选人有更多的了解。家庭和社区特征也预测了一些结果变量。例如,正如预期的那样,如果儿童认为他们的父母支持他,并且如果特朗普在他们居住的县获得了更高的选票,那么他们就更有可能支持特朗普。我们的数据表明,公民教育应该扩大和改革。除了解决需要政治解决方案的社会问题外,公民教育还应包括社会群体参与政治的历史,包括有关性别歧视和美国政治历史上妇女选举权运动的信息。为儿童提供与政治目的和价值相关的丰富信息环境,以及进行政治思考和行动的机会和鼓励,可能对青年和他们的国家都有益。

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索