Milward Sophie J, Kita Sotaro, Apperly Ian A
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom.
Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom.
Cognition. 2014 Sep;132(3):269-79. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.04.008. Epub 2014 May 20.
When two adults jointly perform a task, they often show interference effects whereby the other's task interferes with their own performance (Sebanz, Knoblich, & Prinz, 2003). The current study investigated whether these co-representation effects can be observed in young children. This phenomenon can be used as a criterion for adult-like joint action in children, which has been under debate in existing literature due to the difficulty in identifying what mechanisms underlie the behaviours observed (Brownell, 2011). In Experiment 1, two children performed an adapted Bear Dragon task (Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996), where children were required to point to a picture when instructed to do so by one puppet and to inhibit pointing when instructed to by the other. In the Same Task condition, both children in a pair were asked to respond to the same puppet, whereas in the Different Task condition, they were asked to respond to different puppets. Children made more errors in the Different Task condition than the Same Task, suggesting that they were experiencing interference from their partner's task rule. In Experiment 2 children in Different and Same task conditions began with the same task as in Experiment 1 and then switched which puppet to respond to. Switch costs were lower in the Different task condition, consistent with children having already represented the alternative task rule on behalf of their partner during the pre-switch phase. Experiment 3 replicated the effect of Task in a novel computer-based paradigm with children between 4 and 5years, but not younger. These data provide the first direct evidence that children as young as 4years co-represent a partner's task during a joint activity, and that younger children may not be capable of co-representation.
当两个成年人共同执行一项任务时,他们常常会表现出干扰效应,即对方的任务会干扰自己的表现(塞班茨、克诺布利希和普林茨,2003年)。当前的研究调查了这种共同表征效应是否能在幼儿身上观察到。这一现象可作为儿童类似成人的联合行动的一个标准,由于难以确定观察到的行为背后的机制,这一点在现有文献中一直存在争议(布朗内尔,2011年)。在实验1中,两个孩子执行了一项改编后的“熊龙任务”(科查斯卡、默里、雅克、凯尼格和万德吉斯特,1996年),要求孩子们在一个木偶指示时指向一幅图片,而在另一个木偶指示时抑制指向。在“相同任务”条件下,一对中的两个孩子都被要求对同一个木偶做出反应,而在“不同任务”条件下,他们被要求对不同的木偶做出反应。孩子们在“不同任务”条件下比在“相同任务”条件下犯的错误更多,这表明他们受到了伙伴任务规则的干扰。在实验2中,处于“不同任务”和“相同任务”条件下的孩子开始时执行与实验1相同的任务,然后切换要做出反应的木偶。“不同任务”条件下的切换成本更低,这与孩子们在切换前阶段已经代表伙伴表征了替代任务规则一致。实验3在一个新的基于计算机的范式中对4至5岁的儿童重复了任务效应,但年龄更小的儿童则没有。这些数据提供了首个直接证据,表明4岁的儿童在联合活动中会共同表征伙伴的任务,而年龄更小的儿童可能没有共同表征的能力。