Bourrelly Clara, Quinet Julie, Cavanagh Patrick, Goffart Laurent
Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, UMR 7289, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France; and.
Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, UMR 8242, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France.
J Neurophysiol. 2016 Dec 1;116(6):2739-2751. doi: 10.1152/jn.00519.2016. Epub 2016 Sep 28.
An object moving in the visual field triggers a saccade that brings its image onto the fovea. It is followed by a combination of slow eye movements and catch-up saccades that try to keep the target image on the fovea as long as possible. The accuracy of this ability to track the "here-and-now" location of a visual target contrasts with the spatiotemporally distributed nature of its encoding in the brain. We show in six experimentally naive monkeys how this performance is acquired and gradually evolves during successive daily sessions. During the early exposure, the tracking is mostly saltatory, made of relatively large saccades separated by low eye velocity episodes, demonstrating that accurate (here and now) pursuit is not spontaneous and that gaze direction lags behind its location most of the time. Over the sessions, while the pursuit velocity is enhanced, the gaze is more frequently directed toward the current target location as a consequence of a 25% reduction in the number of catch-up saccades and a 37% reduction in size (for the first saccade). This smoothing is observed at several scales: during the course of single trials, across the set of trials within a session, and over successive sessions. We explain the neurophysiological processes responsible for this combined evolution of saccades and pursuit in the absence of stringent training constraints. More generally, our study shows that the oculomotor system can be used to discover the neural mechanisms underlying the ability to synchronize a motor effector with a dynamic external event.
视野中移动的物体触发扫视,将其图像带到中央凹上。随后是缓慢眼动和追赶扫视的组合,这些动作试图尽可能长时间地将目标图像保持在中央凹上。这种追踪视觉目标“此时此地”位置的能力的准确性,与其在大脑中编码的时空分布特性形成对比。我们在六只未经实验训练的猴子身上展示了这种表现是如何在连续的每日实验过程中习得并逐渐发展的。在早期接触阶段,追踪大多是跳跃式的,由相对较大的扫视动作组成,中间夹杂着低眼速阶段,这表明精确的(此时此地)追踪不是自发的,并且注视方向在大多数时间都落后于其位置。在实验过程中,随着追踪速度的提高,由于追赶扫视次数减少25%以及(第一次扫视)大小减小37%,注视更频繁地指向当前目标位置。这种平滑现象在多个尺度上都能观察到:在单次试验过程中、在一个实验阶段内的一组试验中以及在连续的实验阶段中。我们解释了在没有严格训练约束的情况下,负责扫视和追踪这种联合演变的神经生理过程。更一般地说,我们的研究表明,眼动系统可用于发现将运动效应器与动态外部事件同步的能力背后的神经机制。