From the Departments of Epidemiology (E.R.M., T.M.M.) and Biostatistics (R.E.W.), University of California, Los Angeles Fielding School of Public Health; Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics (E.R.M., A.R.M.), University of California, San Francisco; Departments of Epidemiology (A.R.M., L.F.B.) and Social and Behavioral Sciences (L.F.B.), Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston; and School of Social Work (E.L.S.), Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA.
Neurology. 2020 Dec 8;95(23):e3072-e3080. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010989. Epub 2020 Nov 4.
To test the hypothesis that life course patterns of employment, marriage, and childrearing influence later-life rate of memory decline among women, we examined the relationship of work-family experiences between ages 16 and 50 years and memory decline after age 55 years among US women.
Participants were women ages ≥55 years in the Health and Retirement Study. Participants reported employment, marital, and parenthood statuses between ages 16 and 50 years. Sequence analysis was used to group women with similar work-family life histories; we identified 5 profiles characterized by similar timing and transitions of combined work, marital, and parenthood statuses. Memory performance was assessed biennially from 1995 to 2016. We estimated associations between work-family profiles and later-life memory decline with linear mixed-effects models adjusted for practice effects, baseline age, race/ethnicity, birth region, childhood socioeconomic status, and educational attainment.
There were 6,189 study participants (n = 488 working nonmothers, n = 4,326 working married mothers, n = 530 working single mothers, n = 319 nonworking single mothers, n = 526 nonworking married mothers). Mean baseline age was 57.2 years; average follow-up was 12.3 years. Between ages 55 and 60, memory scores were similar across work-family profiles. After age 60, average rate of memory decline was more than 50% greater among women whose work-family profiles did not include working for pay after childbearing, compared with those who were working mothers.
Women who worked for pay in early adulthood and midlife experienced slower rates of later-life memory decline, regardless of marital and parenthood status, suggesting participation in the paid labor force may protect against later-life memory decline.
为了验证这样一个假设,即人生历程中的就业、婚姻和育儿模式会影响女性晚年的记忆衰退速度,我们研究了 16 岁至 50 岁期间的工作-家庭经历与 55 岁后记忆衰退之间的关系,对象为美国女性。
参与者为健康与退休研究中的年龄在 55 岁及以上的女性。参与者报告了 16 岁至 50 岁期间的就业、婚姻和生育状况。序列分析被用来对具有相似工作-家庭生活史的女性进行分组;我们确定了 5 种特征相似的组合工作、婚姻和生育状况的模式。从 1995 年到 2016 年,每隔两年评估一次记忆表现。我们使用线性混合效应模型估计了工作-家庭模式与晚年记忆衰退之间的关联,该模型调整了练习效应、基线年龄、种族/民族、出生地区、儿童期社会经济地位和教育程度。
共有 6189 名研究参与者(n=488 名有工作的非母亲、n=4326 名有工作的已婚母亲、n=530 名有工作的单身母亲、n=319 名无工作的单身母亲、n=526 名无工作的已婚母亲)。平均基线年龄为 57.2 岁;平均随访时间为 12.3 年。在 55 岁至 60 岁之间,不同工作-家庭模式的记忆评分相似。60 岁后,与生育后不从事有薪工作的女性相比,那些有工作的母亲的记忆衰退速度平均快 50%以上。
在成年早期和中年期间有薪工作的女性经历晚年记忆衰退的速度较慢,无论其婚姻和生育状况如何,这表明参与有薪劳动力市场可能有助于预防晚年记忆衰退。