Suppr超能文献

儿童在语言学习中所具有的结构偏向:对家庭手语手势输入的跨文化研究。

Structural biases that children bring to language learning: A cross-cultural look at gestural input to homesign.

作者信息

Flaherty Molly, Hunsicker Dea, Goldin-Meadow Susan

机构信息

Davidson College, Psychology Department, Davidson, NC 28036, United States of America.

The University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, United States of America.

出版信息

Cognition. 2021 Jun;211:104608. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104608. Epub 2021 Feb 10.

Abstract

Linguistic input has an immediate effect on child language, making it difficult to discern whatever biases children may bring to language-learning. To discover these biases, we turn to deaf children who cannot acquire spoken language and are not exposed to sign language. These children nevertheless produce gestures, called homesigns, which have structural properties found in natural language. We ask whether these properties can be traced to gestures produced by hearing speakers in Nicaragua, a gesture-rich culture, and in the USA, a culture where speakers rarely gesture without speech. We studied 7 homesigning children and hearing family members in Nicaragua, and 4 in the USA. As expected, family members produced more gestures without speech, and longer gesture strings, in Nicaragua than in the USA. However, in both cultures, homesigners displayed more structural complexity than family members, and there was no correlation between individual homesigners and family members with respect to structural complexity. The findings replicate previous work showing that the gestures hearing speakers produce do not offer a model for the structural aspects of homesign, thus suggesting that children bring biases to construct, or learn, these properties to language-learning. The study also goes beyond the current literature in three ways. First, it extends homesign findings to Nicaragua, where homesigners received a richer gestural model than USA homesigners. Moreover, the relatively large numbers of gestures in Nicaragua made it possible to take advantage of more sophisticated statistical techniques than were used in the original homesign studies. Second, the study extends the discovery of complex noun phrases to Nicaraguan homesign. The almost complete absence of complex noun phrases in the hearing family members of both cultures provides the most convincing evidence to date that homesigners, and not their hearing family members, are the ones who introduce structural properties into homesign. Finally, by extending the homesign phenomenon to Nicaragua, the study offers insight into the gestural precursors of an emerging sign language. The findings shed light on the types of structures that an individual can introduce into communication before that communication is shared within a community of users, and thus sheds light on the roots of linguistic structure.

摘要

语言输入对儿童语言有直接影响,这使得难以辨别儿童在语言学习中可能带来的任何偏向。为了发现这些偏向,我们转向那些无法习得口语且未接触手语的失聪儿童。这些儿童会做出被称为“自创手势语”的手势,这些手势具有自然语言中所发现的结构特性。我们探究这些特性是否可以追溯到尼加拉瓜(一个手势丰富的文化环境)和美国(一个说话者很少在没有言语时做手势的文化环境)的有听力的说话者所做出的手势。我们研究了尼加拉瓜的7名使用自创手势语的儿童及其有听力的家庭成员,以及美国的4名。正如预期的那样,尼加拉瓜的家庭成员比美国的家庭成员做出更多无言语的手势以及更长的手势串。然而,在两种文化中,使用自创手势语的儿童所展示的结构复杂性都高于家庭成员,并且在结构复杂性方面,个体的自创手势语使用者与家庭成员之间没有相关性。这些发现重复了之前的研究结果,即有听力的说话者做出的手势并不能为自创手势语的结构方面提供模型,从而表明儿童在语言学习中会带来构建或学习这些特性的偏向。该研究还在三个方面超越了当前的文献。首先,它将自创手势语的研究结果扩展到了尼加拉瓜,在那里,使用自创手势语的儿童比美国的同类儿童获得了更丰富的手势模型。此外,尼加拉瓜相对较多的手势数量使得能够利用比最初的自创手势语研究中所使用的更复杂的统计技术。其次,该研究将复杂名词短语的发现扩展到了尼加拉瓜的自创手势语。两种文化中有听力的家庭成员中几乎完全没有复杂名词短语,这提供了迄今为止最有说服力的证据,表明是使用自创手势语的儿童,而不是他们有听力的家庭成员,将结构特性引入了自创手势语。最后,通过将自创手势语现象扩展到尼加拉瓜,该研究为一种新兴手语的手势前身提供了见解。这些发现揭示了在一种交流方式在一群使用者中共享之前,个体能够引入到交流中的结构类型,从而揭示了语言结构的根源。

相似文献

1
Structural biases that children bring to language learning: A cross-cultural look at gestural input to homesign.
Cognition. 2021 Jun;211:104608. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104608. Epub 2021 Feb 10.
2
Communicating about quantity without a language model: number devices in homesign grammar.
Cogn Psychol. 2013 Aug-Sep;67(1-2):1-25. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.05.003. Epub 2013 Jul 17.
3
Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign.
Cognition. 2017 Jan;158:10-27. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.09.012. Epub 2016 Oct 20.
4
Generating a lexicon without a language model: Do words for number count?
J Mem Lang. 2013 Nov 1;69(4). doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.05.004.
6
Universal Constraints on Linguistic Event Categories: A Cross-Cultural Study of Child Homesign.
Psychol Sci. 2023 Mar;34(3):298-312. doi: 10.1177/09567976221140328. Epub 2023 Jan 6.
7
Gesture, sign, and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies.
Behav Brain Sci. 2017 Jan;40:e46. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15001247. Epub 2015 Oct 5.
8
Widening the lens: what the manual modality reveals about language, learning and cognition.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014 Sep 19;369(1651):20130295. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0295.
9
Language Emergence.
Annu Rev Linguist. 2017;3:363-388. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040743.
10
Watching language grow in the manual modality: nominals, predicates, and handshapes.
Cognition. 2015 Mar;136:381-95. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.029. Epub 2014 Dec 26.

引用本文的文献

1
Community interactions and phonemic inventories in emerging sign languages.
Phonology. 2021 Nov;38(4):571-609. doi: 10.1017/S0952675721000336. Epub 2022 Feb 21.
2
Profile of Susan Goldin-Meadow.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024 Dec 3;121(49):e2421379121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2421379121. Epub 2024 Nov 27.
3
Using computational modeling to validate the onset of productive determiner-noun combinations in English-learning children.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024 Dec 10;121(50):e2316527121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2316527121. Epub 2024 Nov 21.
4
Parent American Sign Language skills correlate with child-but not toddler-ASL vocabulary size.
Lang Acquis. 2024;31(2):85-99. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2023.2178312. Epub 2023 Apr 17.
5
Linguacultural and Cognitive Peculiarities of Linguistic Universals.
J Psycholinguist Res. 2024 Jan 21;53(1):3. doi: 10.1007/s10936-024-10050-3.
6
Emergent Morphology in Child Homesign: Evidence from Number Language.
Lang Learn Dev. 2022;18(1):16-40. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1922281. Epub 2021 Jun 7.

本文引用的文献

1
The communicative importance of agent-backgrounding: Evidence from homesign and Nicaraguan Sign Language.
Cognition. 2020 Oct;203:104332. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104332. Epub 2020 Jun 16.
2
Language Emergence.
Annu Rev Linguist. 2017;3:363-388. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040743.
3
Statistical evidence that a child can create a combinatorial linguistic system without external linguistic input: Implications for language evolution.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017 Oct;81(Pt B):150-157. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.016. Epub 2016 Dec 29.
5
The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition.
J Child Lang. 2015 Mar;42(2):239-73. doi: 10.1017/S030500091400049X.
6
Arguing about innateness.
J Child Lang. 2014 Jul;41 Suppl 1:78-92. doi: 10.1017/S0305000914000336.
7
Modeling the emergence of lexicons in homesign systems.
Top Cogn Sci. 2014 Jan;6(1):183-95. doi: 10.1111/tops.12076.
8
Negation, questions, and structure building in a homesign system.
Cognition. 2011 Mar;118(3):398-416. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.017. Epub 2010 Nov 19.
9
Hierarchical structure in a self-created communication system: Building nominal constituents in homesign.
Language (Baltim). 2012 Dec 1;88(4):732-763. doi: 10.1353/lan.2012.0092.
10
The natural order of events: how speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008 Jul 8;105(27):9163-8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710060105. Epub 2008 Jul 1.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验