Lee Chia-Lin, Federmeier Kara D
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, IL 61820, USA.
Brain Res. 2006 Apr 7;1081(1):191-202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.058. Epub 2006 Mar 3.
The goal of this study was to jointly examine the effects of word class, word class ambiguity, and semantic ambiguity on the brain response to words in syntactically specified contexts. Four types of words were used: (1) word class ambiguous words with a high degree of semantic ambiguity (e.g., 'duck'); (2) word class ambiguous words with little or no semantic ambiguity (e.g., 'vote'); (3) word class unambiguous nouns (e.g., 'sofa'); and (4) word class unambiguous verbs (e.g., 'eat'). These words were embedded in minimal phrases that explicitly specified their word class: "the" for nouns (and ambiguous words used as nouns) and "to" for verbs (and ambiguous words used as verbs). Our results replicate the basic word class effects found in prior work (Federmeier, K.D., Segal, J.B., Lombrozo, T., Kutas, M., 2000. Brain responses to nouns, verbs and class ambiguous words in context. Brain, 123 (12), 2552-2566), including an enhanced N400 (250-450 ms) to nouns compared with verbs and an enhanced frontal positivity (300-700 ms) to unambiguous verbs in relation to unambiguous nouns. A sustained frontal negativity (250-900 ms) that was previously linked to word class ambiguity also appeared in this study but was specific to word class ambiguous items that also had a high level of semantic ambiguity; word class ambiguous items without semantic ambiguity, in contrast, were more positive than class unambiguous words in the early part of this time window (250-500 ms). Thus, this frontal negative effect seems to be driven by the need to resolve the semantic ambiguity that is sometimes associated with different grammatical uses of a word class ambiguous homograph rather than by the class ambiguity per se.
本研究的目的是共同考察词类、词类歧义以及语义歧义对大脑在句法明确的语境中对词汇反应的影响。使用了四种类型的词:(1)具有高度语义歧义的词类歧义词(例如“duck”);(2)几乎没有或没有语义歧义的词类歧义词(例如“vote”);(3)词类明确的名词(例如“sofa”);以及(4)词类明确的动词(例如“eat”)。这些词被嵌入到明确指定其词类的最简短语中:名词(以及用作名词的歧义词)用“the”,动词(以及用作动词的歧义词)用“to”。我们的研究结果重复了先前研究(费德迈尔,K.D.,西格尔,J.B.,隆布罗佐,T.,库塔斯,M.,2000年。语境中大脑对名词、动词和类歧义词的反应。《大脑》,123(12),2552 - 2566)中发现的基本词类效应,包括与动词相比,名词诱发的N400(250 - 450毫秒)增强,以及与明确名词相比,明确动词诱发的额部正波增强(300 - 700毫秒)。本研究中还出现了先前与词类歧义相关的持续额部负波(250 - 900毫秒),但该负波特定于同时具有高度语义歧义的词类歧义项;相比之下,没有语义歧义的词类歧义项在这个时间窗口的早期(250 - 500毫秒)比词类明确的词更正向。因此,这种额部负效应似乎是由解决有时与词类歧义同形异义词的不同语法用法相关的语义歧义的需求驱动的,而不是由词类歧义本身驱动的。