Suppr超能文献

众人皆醉我独醒?高估他人兴奋剂使用情况或表明自身涉药?

Comfort in big numbers: Does over-estimation of doping prevalence in others indicate self-involvement?

机构信息

Kingston University, Faculty of Science, School of Life Sciences, Penrhyn Road, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, KT1 2EE, UK.

出版信息

J Occup Med Toxicol. 2008 Sep 5;3:19. doi: 10.1186/1745-6673-3-19.

Abstract

BACKGROUND

The 'False Consensus Effect' (FCE), by which people perceive their own actions as relatively common behaviour, might be exploited to gauge whether a person engages in controversial behaviour, such as performance enhancing drug (PED) use.

HYPOTHESIS

It is assumed that people's own behaviour, owing to the FCE, affects their estimation of the prevalence of that behaviour. It is further hypothesised that a person's estimate of PED population use is a reliable indicator of the doping behaviour of that person, in lieu of self-reports.

TESTING THE HYPOTHESIS

Over- or underestimation is calculated from investigating known groups (i.e. users vs. non-users), using a short questionnaire, and a known prevalence rate from official reports or sample evidence. It is proposed that sample evidence from self-reported behaviour should be verified using objective biochemical analyses.In order to find proofs of concept for the existence of false consensus, a pilot study was conducted. Data were collected among competitive UK student-athletes (n = 124) using a web-based anonymous questionnaire. User (n = 9) vs. non-user (n = 76) groups were established using self-reported information on doping use and intention to use PEDs in hypothetical situations. Observed differences in the mean estimation of doping made by the user group exceeded the estimation made by the non-user group (35.11% vs. 15.34% for general doping and 34.25% vs. 26.30% in hypothetical situations, respectively), thus providing preliminary evidence in support of the FCE concept in relation to doping.

IMPLICATIONS OF THE HYPOTHESIS

The presence of the FCE in estimating doping prevalence or behaviour in others suggests that the FCE based approach may be an avenue for developing an indirect self-report mechanism for PED use behaviour. The method may be successfully adapted to the estimation of prevalence of behaviours where direct self-reports are assumed to be distorted by socially desirable responding. Thus this method can enhance available information on socially undesirable, health compromising behaviour (i.e. PED use) for policy makers and healthcare professionals. The importance of the method lies in its usefulness in epidemiological studies, not in individual assessments.

摘要

背景

“虚假共识效应”(False Consensus Effect,FCE)是指人们认为自己的行为是相对普遍的行为。这种效应可能被用来评估一个人是否从事有争议的行为,例如使用兴奋剂(Performance Enhancing Drug,PED)。

假设

由于 FCE,人们自己的行为会影响他们对该行为普遍程度的估计。进一步假设,一个人对 PED 使用人群的估计是那个人使用兴奋剂行为的可靠指标,而不是自我报告。

测试假设

通过调查已知群体(即使用者与非使用者),使用简短的问卷,并根据官方报告或样本证据中的已知流行率,从高估或低估中计算出来。有人建议,应该使用客观的生化分析来验证自我报告行为的样本证据。为了找到虚假共识存在的概念证明,进行了一项试点研究。使用基于网络的匿名问卷,在英国有竞争力的学生运动员中(n=124)收集数据。使用自我报告的关于兴奋剂使用的信息和在假设情况下使用 PED 的意图,建立使用者(n=9)与非使用者(n=76)组。使用者组对兴奋剂的平均估计与非使用者组相比存在显著差异(一般兴奋剂 35.11%对 15.34%,假设情况下 34.25%对 26.30%),从而为 FCE 概念在兴奋剂方面提供了初步证据支持。

假设的含义

在估计兴奋剂流行率或他人行为时存在 FCE,表明基于 FCE 的方法可能是开发用于 PED 使用行为的间接自我报告机制的途径。该方法可能会成功地适应于那些直接自我报告被社会期望反应扭曲的行为的估计。因此,这种方法可以增强决策者和医疗保健专业人员对社会不可接受、损害健康的行为(即 PED 使用)的可用信息。该方法的重要性在于其在流行病学研究中的有用性,而不在于个体评估。

https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/5acf/2553062/96bbd19606dc/1745-6673-3-19-1.jpg

相似文献

2
Self-admitted behavior and perceived use of performance-enhancing vs psychoactive drugs among competitive athletes.
Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2011 Apr;21(2):224-34. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01041.x.
3
A call for policy guidance on psychometric testing in doping control in sport.
Int J Drug Policy. 2015 Nov;26(11):1130-9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2015.04.022. Epub 2015 May 3.
6
Elite athletes' estimates of the prevalence of illicit drug use: evidence for the false consensus effect.
Drug Alcohol Rev. 2012 Jan;31(1):27-32. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00307.x. Epub 2011 Mar 31.
8
Analgesics use in competitive triathletes: its relationship to doping and on predicting its usage.
J Sports Sci. 2016 Oct;34(20):1965-9. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1149214. Epub 2016 Feb 25.
9
Randomized response estimates for doping and illicit drug use in elite athletes.
Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010 Jan 15;106(2-3):230-2. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.07.026. Epub 2009 Sep 8.

引用本文的文献

1
Relationship between Doping Prevalence and Socioeconomic Parameters: An Analysis by Sport Categories and World Areas.
Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022 Jul 30;19(15):9329. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159329.
2
Doping knowledge, attitudes, and practices of Ugandan athletes': a cross-sectional study.
Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2015 Sep 22;10:37. doi: 10.1186/s13011-015-0033-2.
3
Prevalence of doping use in elite sports: a review of numbers and methods.
Sports Med. 2015 Jan;45(1):57-69. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0247-x.
4
Personal and psychosocial predictors of doping use in physical activity settings: a meta-analysis.
Sports Med. 2014 Nov;44(11):1603-24. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0240-4.
6
Attitudes towards doping and related experience in Spanish national cycling teams according to different Olympic disciplines.
PLoS One. 2013 Aug 21;8(8):e70999. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070999. eCollection 2013.
7
Doping in sport: a review of elite athletes' attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge.
Sports Med. 2013 Jun;43(6):395-411. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0037-x.
8
Innovative techniques for estimating illegal activities in a human-wildlife-management conflict.
PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e53681. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053681. Epub 2013 Jan 16.
9
Identifying indicators of illegal behaviour: carnivore killing in human-managed landscapes.
Proc Biol Sci. 2012 Feb 22;279(1729):804-12. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1228. Epub 2011 Jul 27.

本文引用的文献

1
Psychological drivers in doping: the life-cycle model of performance enhancement.
Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2008 Mar 10;3:7. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-3-7.
2
Misperceptions of non-medical prescription drug use: a web survey of college students.
Addict Behav. 2008 May;33(5):713-24. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.008. Epub 2007 Dec 27.
3
Performance enhancement with supplements: incongruence between rationale and practice.
J Int Soc Sports Nutr. 2007 Nov 12;4:19. doi: 10.1186/1550-2783-4-19.
5
Sexual behaviors and their correlates among young people in Mauritius: a cross-sectional study.
BMC Int Health Hum Rights. 2007 Oct 5;7:8. doi: 10.1186/1472-698X-7-8.
6
Evaluation of the IDS One-Step ELISA kits for the detection of illicit drugs in hair.
Forensic Sci Int. 2007 Aug 6;170(2-3):189-92. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2007.02.032. Epub 2007 Jul 12.
7
Implications of the normative fallacy in young adult smokers aged 19-24 years.
Am J Public Health. 2007 Aug;97(8):1399-400. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.101071. Epub 2007 Jun 28.
8
Current anti-doping policy: a critical appraisal.
BMC Med Ethics. 2007 Mar 29;8:2. doi: 10.1186/1472-6939-8-2.
9
Doping in fitness sports: estimated number of unreported cases and individual probability of doping.
Addiction. 2006 Nov;101(11):1640-4. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01568.x.
10
Doping use among tertiary education students in six developed countries.
Eur J Epidemiol. 2006;21(4):307-13. doi: 10.1007/s10654-006-0018-6.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验