Suppr超能文献

视觉运动适应泛化的时间稳定性。

The temporal stability of visuomotor adaptation generalization.

作者信息

Zhou Weiwei, Fitzgerald Justin, Colucci-Chang Katrina, Murthy Karthik G, Joiner Wilsaan M

机构信息

Sensorimotor Integration Laboratory, Department of Bioengineering, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia.

Sensorimotor Integration Laboratory, Department of Bioengineering, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia;

出版信息

J Neurophysiol. 2017 Oct 1;118(4):2435-2447. doi: 10.1152/jn.00822.2016. Epub 2017 Aug 2.

Abstract

Movement adaptation in response to systematic motor perturbations exhibits distinct spatial and temporal properties. These characteristics are typically studied in isolation, leaving the interaction largely unknown. Here we examined how the temporal decay of visuomotor adaptation influences the spatial generalization of the motor recalibration. First, we quantified the extent to which adaptation decayed over time. Subjects reached to a peripheral target, and a rotation was applied to the visual feedback of the unseen motion. The retention of this adaptation over different delays (0-120 s) ) decreased by 29.0 ± 6.8% at the longest delay and ) was represented by a simple exponential, with a time constant of 22.5 ± 5.6 s. On the basis of this relationship we simulated how the spatial generalization of adaptation would change with delay. To test this directly, we trained additional subjects with the same perturbation and assessed transfer to 19 different locations (spaced 15° apart, symmetric around the trained location) and examined three delays (~4, 12, and 25 s). Consistent with the simulation, we found that generalization around the trained direction (±15°) significantly decreased with delay and distance, while locations >60° displayed near-constant spatiotemporal transfer. Intermediate distances (30° and 45°) showed a difference in transfer across space, but this amount was approximately constant across time. Interestingly, the decay at the trained direction was faster than that based purely on time, suggesting that the spatial transfer of adaptation is modified by concurrent passive (time dependent) and active (movement dependent) processes. Short-term motor adaptation exhibits distinct spatial and temporal characteristics. Here we investigated the interaction of these features, utilizing a simple motor adaptation paradigm (recalibration of reaching arm movements in response to rotated visual feedback). We examined the changes in the spatial generalization of motor adaptation for different temporal manipulations and report that the spatiotemporal generalization of motor adaptation is generally local and is influenced by both passive (time dependent) and active (movement dependent) learning processes.

摘要

响应系统性运动扰动的运动适应表现出独特的空间和时间特性。这些特性通常是单独研究的,其相互作用在很大程度上尚不清楚。在这里,我们研究了视觉运动适应的时间衰减如何影响运动重新校准的空间泛化。首先,我们量化了适应随时间衰减的程度。受试者伸向一个外周目标,并且对不可见运动的视觉反馈施加一个旋转。这种适应在不同延迟(0 - 120秒)下的保留率在最长延迟时下降了29.0±6.8%,并且由一个简单指数表示,时间常数为22.5±5.6秒。基于这种关系,我们模拟了适应的空间泛化如何随延迟而变化。为了直接测试这一点,我们用相同的扰动训练了另外的受试者,并评估向19个不同位置(间隔15°,围绕训练位置对称)的转移,并检查了三个延迟(约4、12和25秒)。与模拟结果一致,我们发现围绕训练方向(±15°)的泛化随着延迟和距离显著降低,而大于60°的位置显示出近乎恒定的时空转移。中间距离(30°和45°)在空间上的转移存在差异,但这个量在时间上大致恒定。有趣的是,在训练方向上的衰减比纯粹基于时间的衰减更快,这表明适应的空间转移受到同时存在的被动(时间依赖性)和主动(运动依赖性)过程的影响。短期运动适应表现出独特的空间和时间特征。在这里,我们利用一个简单的运动适应范式(响应旋转视觉反馈对伸展手臂运动进行重新校准)研究了这些特征的相互作用。我们研究了不同时间操作下运动适应的空间泛化变化,并报告运动适应的时空泛化通常是局部的,并且受到被动(时间依赖性)和主动(运动依赖性)学习过程的影响。

相似文献

1
The temporal stability of visuomotor adaptation generalization.
J Neurophysiol. 2017 Oct 1;118(4):2435-2447. doi: 10.1152/jn.00822.2016. Epub 2017 Aug 2.
3
Generalization of reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration at different distances in the workspace.
Exp Brain Res. 2015 Mar;233(3):817-27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4157-9. Epub 2014 Dec 6.
4
Distinct coordinate systems for adaptations of movement direction and extent.
J Neurophysiol. 2017 Nov 1;118(5):2670-2686. doi: 10.1152/jn.00326.2016. Epub 2017 Aug 23.
5
The absence or temporal offset of visual feedback does not influence adaptation to novel movement dynamics.
J Neurophysiol. 2017 Oct 1;118(4):2483-2498. doi: 10.1152/jn.00636.2016. Epub 2017 Aug 9.
6
Adaptation and spatial generalization to a triaxial visuomotor perturbation in a virtual reality environment.
Exp Brain Res. 2019 Mar;237(3):793-803. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-05462-2. Epub 2019 Jan 3.
7
Motor adaptation and generalization of reaching movements using motor primitives based on spatial coordinates.
J Neurophysiol. 2015 Feb 15;113(4):1217-33. doi: 10.1152/jn.00002.2014. Epub 2014 Nov 26.
8
Generalization patterns for reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration differ after visuomotor learning.
J Neurophysiol. 2015 Jul;114(1):354-65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00415.2014. Epub 2015 May 13.
9
Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Around the Intended Movement.
eNeuro. 2016 Apr 29;3(2). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0005-16.2016. eCollection 2016 Mar-Apr.
10
Target size matters: target errors contribute to the generalization of implicit visuomotor learning.
J Neurophysiol. 2016 Aug 1;116(2):411-24. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2015. Epub 2016 Apr 27.

引用本文的文献

1
Tiny visual latencies can profoundly impair implicit sensorimotor learning.
Sci Rep. 2025 May 8;15(1):16084. doi: 10.1038/s41598-025-98652-2.
2
The cerebellum acts as the analog to the medial temporal lobe for sensorimotor memory.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024 Oct 15;121(42):e2411459121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2411459121. Epub 2024 Oct 7.
3
Attention defines the context for implicit sensorimotor adaptation.
bioRxiv. 2024 Sep 3:2024.09.03.611108. doi: 10.1101/2024.09.03.611108.
4
Audiomotor prediction errors drive speech adaptation even in the absence of overt movement.
bioRxiv. 2024 Aug 16:2024.08.13.607718. doi: 10.1101/2024.08.13.607718.
5
Advanced feedback enhances sensorimotor adaptation.
Curr Biol. 2024 Mar 11;34(5):1076-1085.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.01.073. Epub 2024 Feb 22.
8
Concurrent Implicit Adaptation to Multiple Opposite Perturbations.
eNeuro. 2023 Aug 7;10(8). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0066-23.2023. Print 2023 Aug.
9
A double dissociation between savings and long-term memory in motor learning.
PLoS Biol. 2023 Apr 27;21(4):e3001799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001799. eCollection 2023 Apr.

本文引用的文献

1
Implications of plan-based generalization in sensorimotor adaptation.
J Neurophysiol. 2017 Jul 1;118(1):383-393. doi: 10.1152/jn.00974.2016. Epub 2017 Apr 12.
2
Effect of coordinate frame compatibility on the transfer of implicit and explicit learning across limbs.
J Neurophysiol. 2016 Sep 1;116(3):1239-49. doi: 10.1152/jn.00410.2016. Epub 2016 Jun 22.
3
Visuomotor Learning Generalizes Around the Intended Movement.
eNeuro. 2016 Apr 29;3(2). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0005-16.2016. eCollection 2016 Mar-Apr.
4
Cerebellum as a forward but not inverse model in visuomotor adaptation task: a tDCS-based and modeling study.
Exp Brain Res. 2016 Apr;234(4):997-1012. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4523-2. Epub 2015 Dec 26.
5
Between-Trial Forgetting Due to Interference and Time in Motor Adaptation.
PLoS One. 2015 Nov 24;10(11):e0142963. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142963. eCollection 2015.
6
Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning.
J Neurosci. 2015 Jul 1;35(26):9568-79. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5061-14.2015.
7
Three timescales in prism adaptation.
J Neurophysiol. 2015 Jan 1;113(1):328-38. doi: 10.1152/jn.00803.2013. Epub 2014 Oct 8.
8
The effects of training breadth on motor generalization.
J Neurophysiol. 2014 Dec 1;112(11):2791-8. doi: 10.1152/jn.00615.2013. Epub 2014 Sep 10.
9
The generalization of prior uncertainty during reaching.
J Neurosci. 2014 Aug 20;34(34):11470-84. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3882-13.2014.
10
Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task.
J Neurosci. 2014 Feb 19;34(8):3023-32. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014.

文献AI研究员

20分钟写一篇综述,助力文献阅读效率提升50倍。

立即体验

用中文搜PubMed

大模型驱动的PubMed中文搜索引擎

马上搜索

文档翻译

学术文献翻译模型,支持多种主流文档格式。

立即体验